metricas
covid
Buscar en
Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental (English Edition)
Toda la web
Inicio Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental (English Edition) Executive function is affected in autism spectrum disorder, but does not correla...
Journal Information
Vol. 9. Issue 1.
Pages 39-50 (January - March 2016)
Visits
8920
Vol. 9. Issue 1.
Pages 39-50 (January - March 2016)
Original article
Full text access
Executive function is affected in autism spectrum disorder, but does not correlate with intelligence
La función ejecutiva está alterada en los trastornos del espectro autista, pero esta no correlaciona con la inteligencia
Visits
8920
Jessica Merchán-Naranjoa,
Corresponding author
jmerchan@iisgm.com

Corresponding author.
, Leticia Boadaa, Ángel del Rey-Mejíasa,b, María Mayorala, Cloe Llorentea, Celso Arangoa, Mara Parelladaa
a Servicio de Psiquiatría del Niño y del Adolescente, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Gregorio Marañón (IiSGM), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain
b Departamento de Metodología, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
This item has received
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Figures (1)
Tables (5)
Table 1. Cognitive domains and raw scores for autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability and healthy controls.
Table 2. Demographic data.
Table 3. Z-scores on neuropsychological tests for autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability compared with healthy controls.
Table 4. Bivariate correlation between intelligence quotients and executive function domains in autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability.
Table 5. Bivariate correlation between neuropsychological and clinical variables in autism spectrum disorders.
Show moreShow less
Abstract
Introduction

Studies of executive function in autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability (ASD-WID) patients are contradictory. We assessed a wide range of executive functioning cognitive domains in a sample of children and adolescents with ASD-WID and compared them with age-, sex-, and intelligence quotient (IQ)-matched healthy controls.

Methods

Twenty-four ASD-WID patients (mean age 12.8±2.5 years; 23 males; mean IQ 99.20±18.81) and 32 healthy controls (mean age 12.9±2.7 years; 30 males; mean IQ 106.81±11.02) were recruited.

Results

Statistically significant differences were found in all cognitive domains assessed, with better performance by the healthy control group: attention (U=185.0; p=0.0005; D=0.90), working memory (T51.48=2.597; p=0.006; D=0.72), mental flexibility (U=236.0; p=0.007; D=0.67), inhibitory control (U=210.0; p=0.002; D=0.71), and problem solving (U=261.0; p=0.021; D=0.62). These statistically significant differences were also found after controlling for IQ.

Conclusion

Children and adolescents with ASD-WID have difficulties transforming and mentally manipulating verbal information, longer response latency, attention problems (difficulty set shifting), trouble with automatic response inhibition and problem solving, despite having normal IQ. Considering the low executive functioning profile found in those patients, we recommend a comprehensive intervention including work on non-social problems related to executive cognitive difficulties.

Keywords:
Asperger syndrome
High functioning autism
Neuropsychological assessment
Intelligence
Executive function
Resumen
Introducción

Los estudios reflejan datos contradictorios sobre un posible deterioro en el funcionamiento ejecutivo en niños y adolescentes con trastorno del espectro autista sin discapacidad intelectual (TEA-SDI). El objetivo del estudio es evaluar el perfil cognitivo de funcionamiento ejecutivo en niños y adolescentes con TEA-SDI y compararlo con el de controles sanos pareados en sexo, edad, estatus socioeconómico, nivel educacional y cociente intelectual (CI).

Métodos

Veinticuatro pacientes con TEA-SDI (edad media 12,8±2,5 años; 23 varones; media de CI 99,20±18,81) y 32 controles (edad media 12,9±2,7 años; 30 varones; media de CI 106,81±11,02) fueron seleccionados.

Resultados

Se encontraron diferencias estadísticamente significativas en todos los dominios cognitivos evaluados a favor de un mejor rendimiento por parte del grupo control: atención (U=185,0; p=0,0005; D=0,90), memoria de trabajo (T51,48=2,597; p=0,006; D=0,72), flexibilidad cognitiva (U=236,0; p=0,007; D=0,67), control inhibitorio (U=210,0; p=0,002; D=0,71) y solución de problemas (U=261,0; p=0,021; D=0,62). Estas diferencias se mantuvieron cuando se realizaron los análisis controlando por CI.

Conclusión

Los niños y adolescentes con TEA-SDI tienen dificultades para transformar y manipular mentalmente información verbal, presentan latencias de respuesta mayores, problemas atencionales (dificultades en el cambio del set), problemas en la inhibición de respuestas automáticas, así como en la solución de problemas, a pesar de tener un CI normal. Teniendo en cuenta las dificultades en funcionamiento ejecutivo de estos pacientes, se recomienda una intervención integral, que incluya el trabajo en este tipo de dificultades.

Palabras clave:
Síndrome de Asperger
Autismo de alto funcionamiento
Evaluación neuropsicológica
Inteligencia
Funcionamiento ejecutivo
Full Text
Introduction

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have social communication deficits starting early in life and stereotyped, repetitive behaviors and/or hyper/hyporeactivity to sensory input.

The relationship between executive function (EF) and autism is much debated.1 The literature yields contradictory results about impairment or normal performance in ASD of attention,1–8 working memory,8–14 mental flexibility,2,4,8,15 inhibitory control,3–8,15–18 and problem solving tasks,4,19 all of which are generally considered part of EF. However, EF is not a unitary construct.20 The term EF often includes a set of cognitive processes such as planning, working memory, attention, problem solving, verbal reasoning, inhibition, mental flexibility, multi-tasking, and initiation and monitoring of actions,21 another way of putting it is that it is an umbrella term for neurologically-based skills involving mental control and self-regulation. In short, EFs are the higher order control processes necessary to guide behavior.22

We aimed at studying EF in patients with high functioning autism and comparing this EF with a measure of general intelligence, with the ultimate goal of reporting empirical information about the non-social deficits of this population; this would help designing appropriate neuropsychological rehabilitation programs. The specific objectives of the study were: to evaluate the EF of a homogeneous sample of children and adolescents with ASD without intellectual disability (ASD-WID), using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, and compare their scores with those of a group of matched healthy controls (HC). We also wanted to explore whether deficits in EF correlated with daily functioning or severity of the disorder.

Our hypothesis was that children and adolescents with ASD-WID would have significantly lower scores on a range of EF tasks, i.e., attention, working memory, mental flexibility, inhibitory, control and problem solving, than an HC group matched for age, education years, and intelligence quotient (IQ).

MethodsParticipants

ASD patient recruitment was conducted at the outpatient Child and Adolescent Department of Psychiatry, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Madrid, Spain between May 2005 and February 2009.23 The HC group was recruited in schools with socio-demographic characteristics similar to patients (from schools in the same area and of similar socioeconomic status).

The inclusion criteria for this study were as follows: (a) age between 7 and 18 years of age; (b) Spanish as a first mother tongue; (c) diagnosis of ASD-WID (defined in the DSM-IV as Asperger syndrome (AS)) or high functioning autism (HFA), (d) availability of a complete EF assessment. The exclusion criteria were: (a) comorbid Axis I disorder at the time of enrollment; (b) history of head injury with loss of consciousness; (c) intelligence quotient <70; (d) significant disease unrelated to ASD; (e) pregnancy and lactation; and (f) substance abuse or dependence. In the HC group, inclusion and exclusion criteria were the same, with the exception of the presence of ASD.

After receiving a full explanation of the study, all parents or legal guardians gave written informed consent, and patients gave their assent to participate. The study was approved by the Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón Clinical Research Ethics Committee.

MeasuresDemographic and clinical data

Demographic data were collected using a structured interview with the participants and their parents or legal guardians. The Hollingshead-Redlich Scale24 was assessed to calculate parental socioeconomic status (SES).

All ASD diagnoses were made by child psychiatrists. The diagnosis procedure included a full developmental medical and psychiatric history and observation in a clinical setting. A gold standard diagnostic procedure was performed by psychiatrists of the Hospital Gregorio Marañón Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department with more than 10 years of experience. The criteria of the Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV)25 and Gillberg criteria26 were used; the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G)27 was administered by ADOS research-certified clinicians when DSM-IV criteria and Gillberg criteria were inconsistent (10 patients with ASD (35.7%)). The Kiddie-SADS-Present and Lifetime Version interview (K-SADS)28 was administered to rule out comorbid psychiatric disorders in the ASD group and psychiatric conditions in HC group. Medication was recorded.

Psychosocial functioning was assessed with the Children's Global Assessment of Functioning (C-GAS) scale,29 Spanish version.30 C-GAS scores >70 are considered good.31

Severity of global symptomatology was assessed with the Clinical Global Impression Scale (CGI)32 in the ASD group.

Intelligence quotient assessment

Given that Estimated Intelligence Quotient (EIQ) is not a reliable measurement in patients with ASD-WID,33 the Full Intelligence Quotient test (FIQ) was administered to the ASD group, using the Spanish translation of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults-third edition (WAIS-III) or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R),34,35 according to age.

Intelligence profiles were estimated in the HC group using Vocabulary and Block Design subtests36 of the WAIS-III or WISC-R.

Executive function assessment

EF was assessed using a neuropsychological battery composed of the following tests:

Stroop Color and Word Test (Stroop), whose reliability has proven very consistent (ranging from 0.73 to 0.86).37,38 This is a classic verbal response inhibition task.

Trail Making Test (TMT),39 whose reliability in clinical groups ranges from 0.69 to 0.94 for part A and from 0.66 to 0.86 for part B.40 TMT is a test of processing speed and set shifting. In this test we calculated the following score: derived score from TMT(B)=(time to complete TMT(B)time to complete TMT(A))/time to complete TMT(A).

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST-table version) for which generalizability of the different component variables range from 0.52 to 0.71.41 WCST is a measure of concept generation, cognitive set shifting, ability to inhibit prepotent responses, attribute identification, abstract reasoning, hypothesis testing and problem solving, and sustained attention.42

Continuous Performance Test-II (CPT-II), for which internal consistency of the different component variables ranges from 0.83 to 0.94.43 This measures sustained attention, impulse control, and information processing speed.

Digit span and Letter-Number sequencing (WAIS-III), with a validity and reliability index of 0.82 and from 0.60 to 0.80, respectively.34

Digit Span Forward is an attention task and Digit Span Backward is a verbal working memory task that measures the ability to transform and mentally manipulate information. We used the span obtained by the participant in this subtest.

Letter-Number sequencing is a working memory task that measures the ability to transform and mentally manipulate information. We used the span obtained by the participant in this subtest.

Five domains of EF (attention, working memory, mental flexibility, inhibitory control, and problem solving) were constructed by selected individual measures from the aforesaid tests. Decisions about grouping individual neuropsychological measurements of each test into cognitive domains (Table 1) were based on the cognitive functions assessed by the tests.44–46

Table 1.

Cognitive domains and raw scores for autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability and healthy controls.

Neuropsychological variable  ASD  HC 
  No.=24  No.=32 
  Mean (SD)  Mean (SD) 
  [range]  [range] 
Cognitive domains
Attention
WAIS-III digits forwarda  5.1 (0.9)  5.8 (1.6) 
  [4 to 7]  [3 to 9] 
Number of correct responses CPT  299.5 (24.8)  312.3 (19.5) 
  [218 to 323]  [231 to 328] 
Mean hit reaction time CPTc  0.6 (0.3)  0.5 (0.2) 
  [0.32 to 1.41]  [0.25 to 1.13] 
Working memory
WAIS-III digits backwarda  4.1 (0.9)  5.03 (1.57) 
  [2 to 6]  [2 to 9] 
WAIS-III number-letter sequencinga  4.7 (0.1)  5.2 (1.2) 
  [2 to 7]  [3 to 8] 
Mental flexibility
Derived score from TMT-Bb,c  1.3 (0.7)  1.3 (0.7) 
  [0.13 to 2.62]  [0.09 to 2.98] 
WCST number of perseverative errorsc  22.6 (15.4)  14.0 (9.9) 
  [3 to 77]  [3 to 45] 
WCST number of errorsc  45.8 (22.1)  28.5 (19.2) 
  [12 to 96]  [7 to 85] 
WCST number of perseverative responsesc  27.3 (20.5)  16.0 (11.6) 
  [3 to 101]  [3 to 52] 
Inhibitory control
Commissions CPTc  23.1 (8.6)  17.3 (8.4) 
  [2 to 34]  [1 to 35] 
Stroop interference score  1.0 (5.2)  3.3 (8.8) 
  [−6.99 to −13.80]  [−27 to −18.72] 
Solving problem
WCST conceptual level responses  57.46 (16.54)  63.53 (14.79) 
  [3 to 84]  [13 to 89] 
WCST number of categories completed  4.29 (1.73)  5.53 (1.02) 
  [0 to 6]  [2 to 6] 
WCST number correct responses  69.04 (12.14)  71.81 (10.44) 
  [32 to 86]  [43 to 97] 

ASD, Autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability; CPT-II, continuous performance test-II; HC, healthy controls; SD, standard deviation; TMT (A–B), trail making test (parts A and B); WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

a

Number of longest series archived (span).

b

Score used in this study: (time to complete TMT(B)time to complete TMT(A))/time to complete TMT(A).

c

Higher raw score was indicative of poorer performance.

In order to obtain summary scores for each cognitive domain, individual raw scores obtained in each neuropsychological measure were transformed to z-scores (mean=0±1) based on the performance of the HC (using controls’ mean and standard deviation raw scores from each neuropsychological measure according to the formula: (z=X1−X¯2/o˜2); where X1 is the raw score of each ASD patient on the neuropsychological measure, and X¯2 and o˜2 were the HC mean and standard deviation raw score from each neuropsychological measure, respectively). The mean summary scores were calculated as the arithmetic means of the individual measurements that composed the specific cognitive domains (mean of the z-scores). All z-scores were calculated in such a way that higher scores always reflected better performance. For cognitive measures where a higher raw score was indicative of poorer performance (derived score from TMT-B, WCST number of perseverative errors, WCST number of errors, WCST number of perseverative responses, CPT error commission, CPT mean hit reaction time), the z-score sign was changed from plus to minus, and vice versa. Z-scores for individual neuropsychological measures grouped into a given functional domain were averaged to establish a summary score for each domain.

The neuropsychological battery was administered to ASD patients and HC in a quiet room. Tasks were presented in a pre-established order. All tests were administrated and scored according to published instructions by one of three experienced neuropsychologists. Prior to the study, all the neuropsychologists demonstrated good inter-rater reliability in administering and scoring all neuropsychological tests. Inter-rater reliability was calculated for vocabulary subtests (WAIS-III/WISC-R) and WCST by examining 10 cases with interclass correlation coefficients ranging 0.95–1.00 for both tests.

Statistical analysis

For descriptive purposes, mean and standard deviation are provided for continuous variables and discrete variables are expressed as frequencies and percentages.

The discrete variable (sex) was analyzed with a chi-square test (χ2), and a Fisher's exact test was used to compare race and parental socioeconomic status between groups.

After testing the assumptions of the generalized linear model of the quantitative variables (age, education years, neuropsychological measures, cognitive domains, C-GAS, and CGI) were assessed by means of a Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk, Student's t-test, or Mann–Whitney U-test for normally and not normally distributed variables, respectively.

In order to further investigate the relationship between EF and IQ, bivariate correlation analyses between the cognitive domains and FSIQ, verbal-IQ (VIQ), and manipulative-IQ (MIQ) were assessed in ASD using Spearman's or Pearson's coefficients as needed.

Although our sample was matched by IQ, we also evaluated the differences between ASD patients and HC in EF domains with a general linear model analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with group (ASD/HC) as a fixed factor, z-scores of each EF domains as dependent variables, and IQ as a covariate.

To investigate the relationship between cognitive domains and functioning and severity (C-GAS and CGI) in ASD patients, we also used Spearman's or Pearson's coefficients.

Effects sizes were calculated for all statistically significant differences. We reported them with Cohen's d coefficient (according to Cohen, 0.2 is indicative of a small, 0.5 of a medium, and 0.8 of a large effect size). Due to the relatively limited number of patients available and in order to rule out possible type II error, we also calculated statistical power in all variables for which we did not find statistical significance between groups.

A significance p-value threshold was set at <0.05. The analyses were performed using SPSS 18.0 for Windows.

ResultsParticipantsDemographic and clinical data

Twenty-four ASD patients (22 AS and 2 HFA according to DSM-IV criteria) and 32 HC met the inclusion criteria for this study. There were no statistically significant differences in any demographic variable between ASD patients and the HC group (age, education years, sex, race, parental SES). These are shown in Table 2.

Table 2.

Demographic data.

No.  ASD  HC  Statisticsa 
  No.=24  No.=32   
Age (years)
Mean (SD)  12.8 (2.5)  12.9 (2.7)  T54=0.105 ns
[Range]  [8 to 18]  [8 to 18] 
Education (years)
Mean (SD)  6.9 (2.6)  7.6 (2.4)  U=326.5 ns
[Range]  [2 to 11]  [2 to 12] 
Gender No. (%)
Male  23 (95.8)  30 (93.8)  χ12=0.117 ns
Female  1 (4.2)  2 (6.3) 
Race No. (%)
Caucasian  23 (95.8)  29 (90.6)  F2;55=3.14 ns
Hispanic  0 (0.0)  3 (9.4) 
Indian  1 (4.2)  0 (0.0) 
Parental SES No. (%)
V_High level  10 (40.7)  9 (28.1)  F4;55=5.844 ns
IV_High-middle level  1 (4.2)  6 (18.8) 
III_Middle level  5 (20.8)  11 (34.4) 
II_Low-middle level  7 (29.2)  4 (12.5) 
I_Low level  1 (4.2)  2 (6.3) 

ASD, Autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability; HC, healthy controls; SD, standard deviation. SES, parental socioeconomic status, assessed using the Hollinshead Scale (ranging from 1 to 5). A rating of five corresponds to the highest SES and a rating of 1 means the lowest SES; ns, non-significative.

a

Statistics: T-Student (T) or Chi-square (χ2) or Fisher Test (F) according to statistical characteristics of the variables.

Differences were found in C-GAS between ASD and HC (ASD: mean score 53.25±12.8 (95% confidence interval (CI) [47.9–58.7]), range 35–85, and median 50; HC: mean score 92.52±5.07 (95% CI [90.6–94.5]), range 75–100, and median 91; U: 1.0, p<0.0001).

According to the CGI score, 8 patients (33.3%) were mildly ill, 9 patients (37.5%) were moderately ill, 6 (25%) were markedly ill and 1 (4.2%) was severely ill.

Medication

Eight ASD patients (39.29%) were taking psychopharmacological drugs (one patient was taking aripiprazole; two were taking risperidone; one was taking risperidone and sertraline; one was taking risperidone and fluoxetine; one was taking risperidone, methylphenidate and topiramate; and two were taking methylphenidate only).

Intelligence quotient assessment

The mean EIQ in the HC was 106.81±11.02. In the ASD group, the mean FIQ was 99.20±18.81. No statistically significant differences were found between groups.

Executive function assessment

The mean raw scores, standard deviation, and range for ASD patients and the HC group on each of the cognitive measure are presented in Table 1.

Comparing z-scores between the two groups for the five EF domains assessed (attention, working memory, inhibitory control, mental flexibility, and problem solving) revealed significant differences between patients and controls, with better performance in favor of HC. When we compared the mean z-scores for each individual cognitive measure, we found statistical differences in the following: WAIS-III Digits Forward, CPT correct responses and CPT mean hit reaction time (all measures included Attention domains); WAIS-III Digits Backward (measure included Working Memory domain); WCST number of perseverative errors, WCST number of errors, WCST number of perseverative responses (measures included Mental Flexibility domain); CPT commission errors (measure included Inhibitory Control domain); WCST number of categories completed (measure included Problem Solving domain) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Z-scores on neuropsychological tests for autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability compared with healthy controls.

  ASD  HC  StatisticsaEffects sizebStatistical power
  No.=24  No.=32 
  Mean (SD)  Mean (SD) 
Cognitive domains
Attention  −0.72 (0.86)  0.0008 (0.68)  U=185.0**  0.90  (–) 
WAIS-III digits forwardc  −0.49 (0.6)  0 (1.0)  U=272.0*  0.60  (–) 
Number of correct responses CPT  −0.65 (1.3)  0 (1.0)  U=235.5**  0.57  (–) 
Mean hit reaction time CPTd  0.99 (1.6)  0 (1.0)  U=−218.0**  0.73  (–) 
Working memory  −0.46 (0.52)  −0.001 (0.88)  T51.48=2.597*  0.72  (–) 
WAIS-III digits backwardc  −0.60 (0.6)  0 (0.9)  U=249.5*  0.73  (–) 
WAIS-III number-letter sequencingc  −0.38 (0.8)  0 (1.0)  U=315ns  (–)  0.31 
Mental flexibility  −0.68 (1.14)  0.0001 (0.85)  U=236.0 *  0.67  (–) 
Derived score from TMT-Bd,e  −0.03 (0.9)  0 (1.0)  T54= 0.117ns  (–)  0.05 
WCST number of perseverative errorsd  0.87 (1.6)  0 (1.0)  U=226.5**  0.65  (–) 
WCST number of errorsd  0.90 (1.2)  0 (1.0)  U= 202.5**  0.83  (–) 
WCST number of perseverative responsesd  0.97 (1.8)  0 (0.9)  U=228.5**  0.68  (–) 
Inhibitory control  −0.47 (0.72)  −0.0001 (0.60)  U=210.0*  0.71  (–) 
Commissions CPTd  0.69 (1.0)  0 (1.0)  T53=−2.53**  0.69  (–) 
Stroop interference score  −0.26 (0.6)  0 (1.0)  U=291.0 ns  (–)  0.20 
Problem solving  −0.63(1.20)  0.0005 (0.80)  U=216.0*  0.62  (–) 
WCST conceptual level responses  −0.41 (1.1)  0 (1.0)  U=298.5 ns  (–)  0.30 
WCST number of categories completed  −1.22 (1.7)  0 (0.9)  U=225.0**  0.87  (–) 
WCST number correct responses  −0.27 (1.2)  0 (1.0)  U=352.5 ns  (–)  0.15 

ASD, Autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability; CPT-II, continuous performance test-II; HC, healthy controls; SD, standard deviation; TMT (A–B), trail making test (parts A and B); WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; ns, non-significative.

a

Statistics: T-Student (T) or Mann–Whitney U test (U) according to statistical characteristics of the variables.

b

Effect size: Cohen's D.

c

Number of longest series archived (span).

d

Score used in this study: (time to complete TMT(B)time to complete TIMT(A))/time to complete TMT(A).

e

Calculated score for the study: (time to complete the TMT [B]time to complete the TMT [A])/time to complete the TMT (A).

*

<0.05.

**

<0.008.

For all neuropsychological measures where we did not find statistical significance between groups (WAIS-III Letter-Number sequencing, TMT-B derived score, Stroop interference score, WCST conceptual level responses, and WCST number of correct responses), we calculated the statistical power and it was not higher than 0.31 (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Executive function profiles for autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability patients. Abbreviations: ASD, Autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability; HC, healthy controls.

(0.11MB).

We did not find a statistically significant difference in any EF domain between ASD-WID patients with or without treatment (attention: U=63.0, p=0.951; working memory: T22=1.026, p=0.316; mental flexibility: U=61.0, p=0.854; inhibitory control: U=53.0, p=0.501; problem solving: U=59.500, p=0.783).

Executive function by IQ

Within the ASD group, we did not find statistically significant correlations between FSIQ, VIQ, MIQ, and any of the EF domains assessed except for working memory. More details are shown in Table 4.

Table 4.

Bivariate correlation between intelligence quotients and executive function domains in autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability.

  FSIQ  VIQ  MIQ 
  Rho/r  Rho/r  Rho/r 
Attention  0.40 ns  0.41 ns  0.29 ns 
Working memory  0.41*  0.47*  0.46* 
Mental flexibility  0.13 ns  0.10 ns  0.10 ns 
Inhibitory control  −0.16 ns  −0.19 ns  −0.08 ns 
Solving problem  −0.03 ns  0.12 ns  0.06 ns 

FSIQ, full scale intelligence quotient (WAIS-III or WISC-R); MIQ, manipulative intelligence quotient; VIQ, verbal intelligence quotient; ns, non-significative.

Statistics: Pearson (r) or Spearman (Rho) according to statistical characteristics of the variables.

*

<0.05.

The differences in the EF domains after controlling for IQ were still significant: attention (F(1)=7.495, p=0.007, D=0.13), working memory (F(1)=4.452; p=0.04; D=0.07), mental flexibility (F(1)=4.804; p=0.03 D=0.08), inhibitory control (F(1)=6.638; p=0.013; D=0.11), and problem solving (F(1)=5.247; p=0.02; D=0.09).

Executive function and adaptive functioning

Only the attention domain scores clearly correlate with daily life functioning as measured by the C-GAS. None of the EF domains correlates with severity (CGI). More details are shown in Table 5.

Table 5.

Bivariate correlation between neuropsychological and clinical variables in autism spectrum disorders.

  C-GAS  C-GAS≤70  CGI  CGI+C-GAS≤70 
  Rho/r  Rho/r  Rho  Rho 
Attention  0.46*  0.56**  −0.23 ns  −0.3 ns 
WAIS-III digits forwarda  0.17 ns  0.08 ns  0.02 ns  0.1 ns 
Number of correct responses CPT  0.32 ns  0.39 ns  −0.24 ns  −0.29 ns 
Mean hit reaction time CPT  −0.35 ns  −0.49*  0.17 ns  0.26 ns 
Working memory  0.25 ns  0.4 ns  −0.08 ns  −0.17 ns 
WAIS-III digits backwarda  0.15 ns  0.27 ns  0.03 ns  −0.04 ns 
WAIS-III number-letter sequencing  0.25 ns  0.35 ns  −0.15 ns  −0.22 ns 
Mental flexibility  0.14 ns  0.2 ns  0.008 ns  −0.03 ns 
Derived score from TMT-Bb  0.08 ns  −0.15 ns  −0.15 ns  −0.08 ns 
WCST number of perseverative errors  −0.25 ns  −0.24 ns  0.14 ns  0.13 ns 
WCST number of errors  −0.02 ns  −0.15 ns  −0.02 ns  0.08 ns 
WCST number of perseverative responses  −0.24 ns  −0.26 ns  0.12 ns  0.13 ns 
Inhibitory control  0.12 ns  0.23 ns  −0.16 ns  −0.22 ns 
Commissions CPT  −0.04 ns  −0.09 ns  0.13 ns  0.14 ns 
Stroop interference score  0.22 ns  0.42*  −0.21 ns  −0.29 ns 
Solving problem  −0.13 ns  −0.01 ns  −0.05 ns  −0.05 ns 
WCST conceptual level responses  −0.08 ns  −0.04 ns  0.07 ns  −0.03 ns 
WCST number of categories completed  0.01 ns  0.14 ns  −0.03 ns  −0.13 ns 
WCST number correct responses  −0.39 ns  −0.31 ns  0.31 ns  0.24 ns 

ASD, Autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability; C-GAS, children's global assessment of functioning; CGI, Clinical Global Impression Scale; CGI+C-GAS70, CGI scores in patients with C-GAS70; CPT-II, continuous performance test-II; SD, standard deviation; TMT (A–B), trail making test (parts A and B); WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; ns, non-significative.

a

Number of longest series archived (span).

b

Score used in this study: (time to complete TMT(B)time to complete TMT(A))/time to complete TMT(A).

Statistics: Pearson (r) or Spearman (Rho) according to statistical characteristics of the variables.

*

<0.05.

**

<0.006.

Discussion

Our results show that children and adolescents with ASD-WID have impaired EF. Despite having a normal full IQ, the cognitive profile of our sample is characterized by significant deficits in the five EF domains assessed (attention, working memory, flexibility, inhibitory control, and problem solving). Moreover, the EF results do not correlate with IQ in this sample of ASD patients.

During the past three decades, numerous studies support an executive impairment in autism.47–56 However, most studies have reported a more limited evaluation of executive functioning. Firstly, in the attention domain, our results suggest difficulties in sustained and selective attention tasks, and longer response latency in ASD. Similar problems have been shown in the literature.1–3,57 Kilinçaslan et al. did not find differences between the ASD and control groups in a sustained attention task, but did when the ASD group had a comorbid ADHD.4 It is remarkable that, although none of our patients had a comorbid ADHD disorder, they probably had some subthreshold attention symptoms, as previous research has demonstrated.58 In fact, given the importance of attention for academic performance, for planning of intervention programs, attention difficulties should be taken into account in child/adolescent ASD populations, even when they do not have a comorbid ADHD diagnosis.

Secondly, problems with inhibition of automatic responses were found in our study, in line with previous studies.3–5,7,8,13,15,18 This deficit shows that ASD patients have difficulties inhibiting prepotent responses, implying longer times to process information and perform tasks. This result is in line with previous studies in ASD patients, studies that demonstrate processing speed problems across the lifespan in individuals with ASD.59–63 Slower processing speeds have been associated with severe communication symptoms in high functioning children with ASD61 and were found to be predictive of educational achievements in math, reading, and writing.64

Problems with mental flexibility have also previously been reported,4,7,8,15,17,65 with some exceptions, as in Kaland et al., who used a computerized version of the WCST2 to assess mental flexibility in ASD patients. Their results are in line with some studies that had reported better performance with this computerized task in ASD patients.16,66 Poor cognitive flexibility is manifested by perseverative and stereotyped behavior and difficulties in the regulation and modulation of motor acts.48 In fact, although correlations between repetitive behavior, insistence on sameness, and cognitive perseveration have not been clearly established, insistence on sameness is a characteristic that has been included in the core symptoms of autism by some of the most preeminent authors.67–69 In the recently published DSM 5,70 fulfillment of a B criterion that comprises insistence on sameness together with other repetitive or stereotyped behaviors and/or sensory anomalies is mandatory for a diagnosis of ASD.

Furthermore, the deficits in verbal working memory reported in our study are in line with previous studies in the literature that indicate that ASD patients have difficulty retaining and manipulating verbal and visual information.7–9,13–16 Cui et al. assessed and compared verbal and working memory and found children with ASD had better performance in verbal working memory and worse performance in visual working memory tasks, suggesting a possible imbalance of working memory development in ASD children.12 Taking into account these results and Baddeley's model71 of working memory, our data does not help us understand whether patients have a central executive problem that in turn affects both phonological and visuospatial processing, or whether these abnormalities are independent.

Likewise, our ASD patients had difficulty with the problem-solving domain. Along the same lines, Troyb et al. found that HFA were less efficient in planning and problem-solving tasks.72 Some authors explain this impairment as the consequence of deficient global processing in autism.73–75

These results show that ASD-WID patients have important executive dysfunctions. We have also shown that EF dysfunctions do not correlate with IQ in our sample of children and adolescents with ASD. The possible correlation of EF with IQ in ASD patients is under debate.7,76–78 We have previously shown that IQ profiles assessed with the WAIS-III and the WISC-R follow a similar distribution,33 although patients who were administered the WAIS-III obtained a larger number of subtests scores falling below the normal range. In our sample, we found only a positive correlation between intelligence measures and working memory, suggesting higher intelligence scores correlated with better performance in the working memory domain. This result should be interpreted with caution because in our study, the working memory domain was composed of two subtests included in the IQ measures (WAIS-III Digits Backward and WAIS-III Letter-Number Sequencing), which may explain the correlation found. It would be necessary to study this relationship with other working memory tasks in order to confirm or refute the results obtained in our study. Moreover, when ASD-WID patients are compared with HC controlling for IQ, we also found statistically significantly worse performance in the ASD group. In the same vein as our results, in an ASD sample compared with a non-IQ-matched control group, Narzisi et al. found that deficits in EF were still significant after removing the effects of verbal IQ and that neuropsychological linguistic abilities favor a primary dysfunction of EF in ASD-WID.7 In their study, they used the NEPSY-II battery79 to explore the neuropsychological profile in ASD-WID, which includes EF. Although we used a different neuropsychological battery to assess EF in ASD-WID, our results were similar in all domains, even when compared with groups well matched for IQ, as in our study.

A recommendation derived from these results is that comprehensive executive functioning assessments should be conducted as part of the global assessment in ASD patients, as in other psychiatric pathologies,80 especially for the purpose of designing interventions, as measurement of global intelligence is not enough to give an idea of the patient's cognitive abilities. The recent classification of mental disorders (DSM-5) allows for clinical specifiers to be added to the diagnosis of ASD in order to give a more comprehensive picture of the patient clinical profile and to guide interventions. We would argue that an evaluation of executive functioning should be part of the basic comprehensive evaluation for therapeutic purposes; having only data on general cognitive functioning may overestimate the capacity of ASD subjects. A neuropsychological assessment of EF should include the cognitive domains of attention, working memory (verbal and visual–spatial), inhibitory control, mental flexibility, planning, and problem solving. To ensure that global processing abilities do not interfere with specific EF performance, the specific tasks should be selected according to level of cognitive complexity, considering that some authors have found ASD-WID patients have deficits specifically associated with increasing cognitive load,77,81–84 along with selected computerized tasks, as studies have indicated that ASD patients perform better on computerized tasks.16,66 Our results show no correlation with adaptive functioning or severity as measured by CGAS and CGI. However, these are not subtle measures of academic or vocational performance, very important aspects of long-term prognosis. Therefore, from a clinical perspective, it would be interesting to explore how performance on executive functioning tests correlates with executive functioning questionnaires in reported real life by parents and teachers, such as the behavior rating inventory of EF (BRIEF),85 or with other measures of real-life performance.

Given that ASD are usually diagnosed during childhood development, changes in EF impairment according to age should be considered in treatment plans.7,15,86 Along this line, studies have found age-related differences in working memory, organization, and initiation,86 inhibition, planning, and flexibility86 in children and adolescents with ASD according to parent reports.

The results of the present study should be interpreted with caution due to some methodological limitations. Firstly, the sample size of ASD-WID patients was small. However, we found statistically significant differences among principal study variables (EF domains). Secondly, we used the table version of the WCST, and studies indicate that ASD patients perform better on the WCST using the computer version.16,66 Thirdly, although the cognitive assessment was performed using an extensive EF battery, we did not evaluate visual and spatial working memory, or phonological or semantic fluency. The strengths of this study include an exhaustive assessment of an extensive range of EF domains in a well matched, homogeneous sample of patients with ASD-WID and HC. These five domains were evaluated with a comprehensive neuropsychological battery composed of different tests specifically designed for evaluation of EF.

Future studies should consider examining executive dysfunction in TEA patients who also present with a psychiatric comorbidity.

In conclusion, children and adolescents with ASD-WID have difficulty retaining and mentally manipulating verbal information, longer response latency, attention shifting problems, lack of inhibition of automatic responses, and problem-solving difficulties, despite having a normal IQ. Considering the low EF profile found in our group, we suggest a comprehensive intervention including complementary rehabilitation programs in the non-social domains.

Ethical responsibilitiesProtection of human and animal subjects

The authors declare that the procedures followed were in accordance with the regulations of the responsible Clinical Research Ethics Committee and in accordance with those of the World Medical Association and the Helsinki Declaration.

Confidentiality of data

The authors declare that they have followed the protocols of their work centre on the publication of patient data and that all the patients included in the study have received sufficient information and have given their informed consent in writing to participate in that study.

Right to privacy and informed consent

The authors must have obtained the informed consent of the patients and/or subjects mentioned in the article. The author for correspondence must be in possession of this document.

Funding

This work was carried out with the support of Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, Institute of Salud Carlos III, CIBERSAM, the Community of Madrid (Supports R&D office in Biomedi-S2010/BMD-2422 AGES) and Structural Funds of the European Union, the Alicia Koplowitz Foundation, la Fundación Mutua Madrileña and ERA-NET NEURON (Network of European Funding for Neuroscience Research) Foundation.

Conflict of interest

Jessica Merchán-Naranjo declares no conflict of interest.

Leticia Boada declares no conflict of interest.

Ángel King-Mejias declares no conflict of interest.

María Mayoral has received a research grant from Madrid (predoctoral fellowship) Institute of Carlos III and Health and the Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness.

Cloe Llorente declares no conflict of interest.

Celso Arango has been a consultant and has received fees or grants from Abbot, Amgen, AstraZeneca, Bristol-MyersSquibb, Caja Navarra, CIBERSAM, Alicia Koplowitz Foundation, Institute of Carlos III, Janssen Cilag, Lundbeck, Merck, Ministry of Science and Innovation, Ministry of Sanidad, Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, Mutua Madrileña, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche, Servier, Shire, Schering-Plough and Takeda.

Parellada Mara has received grants for training and travel of the Alicia Koplowitz Foundation and Otsuka.

Acknowledgements

Thanks to the Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, Carlos III Health Institute, CIBERSAM, Madrid and Structural Funds of the European Union, Alicia Koplowitz Foundation and Mutua Madrileña Foundation and ERA-NET NEURON (Network of European Funding for Neuroscience Research).

References
[1]
E.L. Hill, C.M. Bird.
Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: patterns of performance in a multiple case series.
Neuropsychologia, 44 (2006), pp. 2822-2835
[2]
N. Kaland, L. Smith, E.L. Mortensen.
Brief report: cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism as measured on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
J Autism Dev Disord, 38 (2008), pp. 1161-1165
[3]
T. Xiao, Z. Xiao, X. Ke, S. Hong, H. Yang, Y. Su, et al.
Response inhibition impairment in high functioning autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence from near-infrared spectroscopy data.
[4]
A. Kilinçaslan, N. Motavalli Mukaddes, G. Sözen Küçükyazici, H. Gürvit.
Assessment of executive/attentional performance in Asperger's disorder.
Turk Psikiyatri Derg, 21 (2010), pp. 289-299
[5]
M. Katagiri, T. Kasai, Y. Kamio, H. Murohashi.
Individuals with Asperger's disorder exhibit difficulty in switching attention from a local level to a global level.
J Autism Dev Disord, 43 (2013), pp. 395-403
[6]
H. Bogte, B. Flamma, J. van der Meere, H. van Engeland.
Cognitive flexibility in adults with high functioning autism.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 30 (2008), pp. 33-41
[7]
A. Narzisi, F. Muratori, S. Calderoni, F. Fabbro, C. Urgesi.
Neuropsychological profile in high functioning autism spectrum disorders.
J Autism Dev Disord, 43 (2013), pp. 1895-1909
[8]
B.A. Corbett, L.J. Constantine, R. Hendren, D. Rocke, S. Ozonoff.
Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development.
Psychiatry Res, 166 (2009), pp. 210-222
[9]
P.N. Andersen, K.T. Hovik, E.W. Skogli, J. Egeland, M. Oie.
Symptoms of ADHD in children with high-functioning autism are related to impaired verbal working memory and verbal delayed recall.
[10]
M. Goldberg, S. Mostofsky, L. Cutting, E. Mahone, B. Astor, M. Denckla, et al.
Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD.
J Autism Dev Disord, 35 (2005), pp. 279-293
[11]
R. Morris, A. Rowe, N. Fox, J. Feigenbaum, E. Miotto, P. Howlin.
Spatial working memory in Asperger's syndrome and in patients with focal frontal and temporal lobe lesions.
Brain Cogn, 41 (1999), pp. 9-26
[12]
J. Cui, D. Gao, Y. Chen, X. Zou, Y. Wang.
Working memory in early-school-age children with Asperger's syndrome.
J Autism Dev Disord, 40 (2010), pp. 958-967
[13]
H. Geurts, S. Verté, J. Oosterlaan, H. Roeyers, J. Sergeant.
How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism?.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 45 (2004), pp. 836-854
[14]
N. Russo, T. Flanagan, G. Iarocci, D. Berringer, P.D. Zelazo, J.A. Burack.
Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience.
Brain Cogn, 65 (2007), pp. 77-86
[15]
R.L. McLean, A. Johnson Harrison, E. Zimak, R.M. Joseph, E.M. Morrow.
Executive function in probands with autism with average IQ and their unaffected first-degree relatives.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 53 (2014), pp. 1001-1009
[16]
M. Sachse, S. Schlitt, D. Hainz, A. Ciaramidaro, S. Schirman, H. Walter, et al.
Executive and visuo-motor function in adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder.
J Autism Dev Disord, 43 (2013), pp. 1222-1235
[17]
F.Z. Ambery, A.J. Russell, K. Perry, R. Morris, D.G. Murphy.
Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome.
Autism, 10 (2006), pp. 551-564
[18]
C. Sanderson, M.L. Allen.
The specificity of inhibitory impairments in autism and their relation to ADHD-type symptoms.
J Autism Dev Disord, 43 (2013), pp. 1065-1079
[19]
E. Troyb, A. Orinstein, K. Tyson, M. Helt, I.M. Eigsti, M. Stevens, et al.
Academic abilities in children and adolescents with a history of autism spectrum disorders who have achieved optimal outcomes.
Autism, 18 (2014), pp. 233-243
[20]
D.V. Bishop, C.F. Norbury.
Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology. 2. Response inhibition.
[21]
R.C. Chan, D. Shum, T. Toulopoulou, E.Y. Chen.
Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol, 23 (2008), pp. 201-216
[22]
M.B. Jurado, M. Rosselli.
The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding.
Neuropsychol Rev, 17 (2007), pp. 213-233
[23]
M. Parellada, L. Boada, C. Moreno, C. Llorente, J. Romo, C. Muela, et al.
Specialty Care Programme for autism spectrum disorders in an urban population: a case-management model for health care delivery in an ASD population.
Eur Psychiatry, 28 (2013), pp. 102-109
[24]
A.B. Hollingshead, F.F.C. Redlich.
Social class and mental illness: a community study.
John Wiley & Sons, (1958),
[25]
American Psychiatric Association.
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV.
4th ed., American Psychiatric Association, (1994),
[26]
I.C. Gillberg, C. Gillberg.
Asperger syndrome – some epidemiological considerations: a research note.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 30 (1989), pp. 631-638
[27]
C. Lord, M. Rutter, S. Goode, J. Heemsbergen, H. Jordan, L. Mawhood, et al.
Austism diagnostic observation schedule: a standardized observation of communicative and social behavior.
J Autism Dev Disord, 19 (1989), pp. 185-212
[28]
J. Kaufman, B. Birmaher, D. Brent, U. Rao, C. Flynn, P. Moreci, et al.
Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 36 (1997), pp. 980-988
[29]
D. Shaffer, M.S. Gould, J. Brasic, P. Ambrosini, P. Fisher, H. Bird, et al.
A Children's Global Assessment Scale (CGAS).
Arch Gen Psychiatry, 40 (1983), pp. 1228-1231
[30]
L. Ezpeleta, R. Granero, N. de la Osa.
Evaluación del deterioro en niños y adolescentes a través de la Children's Global Assessment Scale (CGAS).
RPIJ, 1 (1999), pp. 18-26
[31]
H. Remschmidt, M. Martin, C. Fleischhaker, F.M. Theisen, K. Hennighausen, C. Gutenbrunner, et al.
Forty-two-years later: the outcome of childhood-onset schizophrenia.
J Neural Transm (Vienna), 114 (2007), pp. 505-512
[32]
W. Guy.
ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology.
U.S. Department of Heath, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute of Mental Health, Psychopharmacology Research Branch, Division of Extramural Research Programs, (1976),
[33]
J. Merchán-Naranjo, M. Mayoral, M. Rapado-Castro, C. Llorente, L. Boada, C. Arango, et al.
Estimation of the intelligence quotient using Wechsler Intelligence Scales in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome.
J Autism Dev Disord, 42 (2012), pp. 116-122
[34]
D. Wechsler.
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Administration and scoring manual.
3rd ed. (WAIS-III), Psychological Corporation, (1997),
[35]
D. Wechsler.
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R).
Psychological Corporation, (1974),
[36]
J.M. Sattler.
Assessment of children cognitive applications.
Jerome M, Sattler Publisher Inc., (2001),
[37]
C. Golden.
STROOP: Test de colores y palabras.
TEA Ediciones, SA, (1994),
[38]
C.J. Golden.
A group version of the Stroop Color and Word Test.
J Personal Assess, 39 (1975), pp. 386-388
[39]
R. Reitan.
Validity of the trail making test as an indication of organic brain damages.
Percept Mot Skills, 8 (1959), pp. 271-276
[40]
G. Goldstein, J.R. Watson.
Test–retest reliability of the Halstein-Reitan neuropsychological battery and the WAIS in the neuropsychiatric population.
Clin Neuropsychol, 3 (1989), pp. 265-273
[41]
R. Heaton, G. Chelune, J. Talley, G. Kay, G. Curtiss.
WCST: test de clasificacion de tarjetas de Wisconsin.
TEA Ediciones, (2001),
[42]
I. Sue-Baron.
Neuropsychological evaluation of the child.
Oxford University Press, (2004),
[43]
C.K. Conners, M.H.S. Staff.
Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II).
Multi-Health Systems Inc., (2000),
[44]
M. Lezak.
Neuropsychological assessment.
Oxford University Press, (1995),
[45]
E. Strauss, E. Sherman, O. Spreen.
A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration norms and commentary.
Oxford University Press, (2006),
[46]
E.L. Hill.
Executive dysfunction in autism.
Trends Cogn Sci, 8 (2004), pp. 26-32
[47]
E.L. Hill.
Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism.
Dev Rev, 24 (2004), pp. 189-233
[48]
S. Ozonoff, B.F. Pennington, S.J. Rogers.
Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 32 (1991), pp. 1081-1105
[49]
C. Hughes, J. Russell.
Autistic children's difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: its implications for theories of autism.
Dev Psychol, 29 (1993), pp. 498-510
[50]
S. Ozonoff, D.L. Strayer, W.M. McMahon, F. Filloux.
Executive function abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: an information processing approach.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 35 (1994), pp. 1015-1032
[51]
B.F. Pennington, S. Ozonoff.
Executive functions and developmental psychopathology.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 37 (1996), pp. 51-87
[52]
M. Prior, W. Hoffmann.
Brief report: neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests.
J Autism Dev Disord, 20 (1990), pp. 581-590
[53]
J.M. Rumsey, S.D. Hamburger.
Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol, 10 (1988), pp. 201-221
[54]
E.L. Hill, U. Frith.
Understanding autism: insights from mind and brain.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B: Biol Sci, 358 (2003), pp. 281-289
[55]
H. Geurts, B. Corbett, M. Solomon.
The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism.
Trends Cogn Sci, 13 (2009), pp. 74-82
[56]
M. McGonigle-Chalmers, K. Bodner, A. Fox-Pitt, L. Nicholson.
Size sequencing as a window on executive control in children with autism and Asperger's syndrome.
J Autism Dev Disord, 38 (2008), pp. 1382-1390
[57]
M. Caamaño, L. Boada, J. Merchán-Naranjo, C. Moreno, C. Llorente, D. Moreno, et al.
Psychopathology in children and adolescents with ASD without mental retardation.
J Autism Dev Disord, 43 (2013), pp. 2442-2449
[58]
S.D. Mayes, S.L. Calhoun.
Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford-Binet:IV, and academic achievement test scores in children with autism.
J Autism Dev Disord, 33 (2003), pp. 329-341
[59]
S.D. Mayes, S.L. Calhoun.
WISC-IV and WIAT-II profiles in children with high-functioning autism.
J Autism Dev Disord, 38 (2008), pp. 428-439
[60]
R.E. Oliveras-Rentas, L. Kenworthy, R.B. Roberson 3rd, A. Martin, G.L. Wallace.
WISC-IV profile in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: impaired processing speed is associated with increased autism communication symptoms and decreased adaptive communication abilities.
J Autism Dev Disord, 42 (2012), pp. 655-664
[61]
A. Spek, T. Schatorjé, E. Scholte, I. van Berckelaer-Onnes.
Verbal fluency in adults with high functioning autism or Asperger syndrome.
Neuropsychologia, 47 (2009), pp. 652-656
[62]
D. Wechsler.
WISC-IV technical and interpretive manual.
Psychological Corporation, (2003),
[63]
S.G. Assouline, M. Foley Nicpon, L. Dockery.
Predicting the academic achievement of gifted students with autism spectrum disorder.
J Autism Dev Disord, 42 (2012), pp. 1781-1789
[64]
Y. Yasuda, R. Hashimoto, K. Ohi, H. Yamamori, M. Fujimoto, S. Umeda-Yano, et al.
Cognitive inflexibility in Japanese adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorders.
World J Psychiatry, 4 (2014), pp. 42-48
[65]
S. Ozonoff.
Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Neuropsychology, 9 (1995), pp. 491-500
[66]
F. Happe, U. Frith.
The neuropsychology of autism.
Brain, 119 (1996), pp. 1377-1400
[67]
L. Wing, J. Gould.
Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification.
J Autism Dev Disord, 9 (1979), pp. 11-29
[68]
L. Kanner.
Autistic disturbances of affective contact.
Acta Paedopsychiatr, 35 (1968), pp. 100-136
[69]
American Psychiatric Association.
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
5th ed. (DSM-5), American Psychatric Publishing, (2013),
[70]
A. Baddely.
Working memory.
Science, 255 (1992), pp. 556-559
[71]
E. Troyb, M. Rosenthal, I.M. Eigsti, E. Kelley, K. Tyson, A. Orinstein, et al.
Executive functioning in individuals with a history of ASDs who have achieved optimal outcomes.
Child Neuropsychol, 20 (2014), pp. 378-397
[72]
E.S. Kuschner, K.E. Bodner, N.J. Minshew.
Local vs. global approaches to reproducing the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure by children, adolescents, and adults with high-functioning autism.
Autism Res, 2 (2009), pp. 348-358
[73]
L. Mottron, S. Belleville, E. Menard.
Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: perceptual hierarchization or working memory deficit?.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 40 (1999), pp. 743-755
[74]
L. Mottron, I. Peretz, E. Menard.
Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: beyond central coherence?.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 41 (2000), pp. 1057-1065
[75]
M. Liss, D. Fein, D. Allen, M. Dunn, C. Feinstein, R. Morris, et al.
Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 42 (2001), pp. 261-270
[76]
L. Van Eylen, B. Boets, J. Steyaert, J. Wagemans, I. Noens.
Executive functioning in autism spectrum disorders: influence of task and sample characteristics and relation to symptom severity.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 24 (2015), pp. 1399-1417
[77]
S. Faja, G. Dawson.
Performance on the dimensional change card sort and backward digit span by young children with autism without intellectual disability.
Child Neuropsychol, 20 (2014), pp. 692-699
[78]
C. Urgesi, F. Campanella, F. Fabbro.
NEPSY-II, contributo alla taratura italiana.
Giunti OS, (2011),
[79]
J.M. Gavilán, J.E. García-Albea.
Executive dysfunction in schizophrenia and its association with mentalizing abilities.
Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment, 8 (2015), pp. 119-129
[80]
N. Rinehart, J. Bradshaw, A. Brereton, B. Tongue.
Lateralization in individuals with high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder: a frontoestriatal model.
J Autism Dev Disord, 32 (2002), pp. 321-332
[81]
D.L. Williams, G. Goldstein, P.A. Carpenter, N.J. Minshew.
Verbal and spatial working memory in autism.
J Autism Dev Disord, 35 (2005), pp. 747-756
[82]
J. Russell, C. Jarrold, L. Henry.
Working memory in children with autism and with moderate learning difficulties.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 37 (1996), pp. 673-686
[83]
S. Ozonoff, D. Strayer.
Further evidence of intact working memory in autism.
J Autism Dev Disord, 31 (2001), pp. 257-263
[84]
G.A. Gioia, P.K. Isquith, P.D. Retzlaff, K.A. Espy.
Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample.
Child Neurophychol, 8 (2002), pp. 249-257
[85]
M. Rosenthal, G.L. Wallace, R. Lawson, M.C. Wills, E. Dixon, B.E. Yerys, et al.
Impairments in real-world executive function increase from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorders.
Neuropsychology, 27 (2013), pp. 13-18
[86]
S.F. Van den Bergh, A.M. Scheeren, S. Begeer, H.M. Koot, H.M. Geurts.
Age related differences of executive functioning problems in everyday life of children and adolescents in the autism spectrum.
J Autism Dev Disord, 44 (2014), pp. 1959-1971

Please cite this article as: Merchán-Naranjo J, Boada L, del Rey-Mejías Á, Mayoral M, Llorente C, Arango C, et al. La función ejecutiva está alterada en los trastornos del espectro autista, pero esta no correlaciona con la inteligencia. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment (Barc). 2016;9:39–50.

Copyright © 2015. SEP and SEPB
Download PDF
Article options
es en pt

¿Es usted profesional sanitario apto para prescribir o dispensar medicamentos?

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?

Você é um profissional de saúde habilitado a prescrever ou dispensar medicamentos