metricas
covid
Buscar en
Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría (English Edition)
Toda la web
Inicio Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría (English Edition) Evaluation of the stigma towards people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia using ...
Información de la revista
Vol. 47. Núm. 2.
Páginas 72-81 (abril - junio 2018)
Visitas
2179
Vol. 47. Núm. 2.
Páginas 72-81 (abril - junio 2018)
Original article
Acceso a texto completo
Evaluation of the stigma towards people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia using a knowledge scale
Evaluación del estigma hacia personas con diagnóstico de esquizofrenia mediante una escala de conocimiento
Visitas
2179
Pamela Grandóna,
Autor para correspondencia
pgrandon@udec.cl

Corresponding author.
, Alexis Vielma Aguilerab, Claudio Bustosb, Elvis Castro Alzatec, Sandra Saldiviab
a Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Concepción, Concepción, Chile
b Departamento de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Concepción, Concepción, Chile
c Escuela de Rehabilitación Humana, Universidad del Valle, Cali, Colombia
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Figuras (1)
Tablas (5)
Table 1. Socio-demographic data for the sample.
Table 2. Item response rates.
Table 3. Results by dimensions. Dimension 1. Trend of knowledge about severe mental illnesses.
Table 4. Results by dimensions. Dimension 2. Trend of attitude assessment on severe mental illnesses.
Table 5. Distribution of socio-demographic variables in the 3 groups, by gender.
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Abstract
Introduction

Social stigma is the assigning of negative stereotypes to people with schizophrenia. Different measurement tools have been used to evaluate this, including knowledge scales. The aim of this study was to evaluate the public stigma by measuring this knowledge and relate the degree of information with variables that have shown to influence on stigma presented by the affected population.

Methods

The sample was composed of 399 people and the inclusion criterion was being between 18 and 65 years of age. The “questionnaire of knowledge on schizophrenia” was applied, as well as a questionnaire to collect sociodemographic information. Participants were recruited in places with large crowds. The following analyses were performed: multiple correlations, non-parametric bivariate and hierarchical clusters.

Results

The questionnaire had two dimensions: “beliefs on the knowledge of schizophrenia” and “attitudes towards schizophrenia”. There are significant differences between them, and the contact with people with SMI. In the analysis of clusters, there was difference in the two groups according to the combination of the two dimensions of the tools.

Conclusions

It is highlighted that none of the dimensions measures true knowledge, and the questionnaire has an attitudinal dimension. More than contact itself, it is the type of interaction of a relevant variable at the level of stigma that questions the traditional hypothesis of contact. Further research is required on the characteristics of the tool and the aspects of the contact associated to a lower level of stigma in the population.

Keywords:
Social stigma
Schizophrenia
Public opinion
Attitude
Resumen
Introducción

El estigma público es la adscripción de estereotipos negativos a las personas con diagnóstico de esquizofrenia. Se han utilizado distintos instrumentos para evaluarlo, entre los que destacan las escalas de conocimiento. El objetivo del estudio es evaluar el estigma público a través de una medida de conocimiento y relacionar el nivel de información con variables con demostrada influencia en el estigma que presenta la población.

Métodos

La muestra fue de 399 personas; el criterio de inclusión fue tener entre 18 y 65 años. Se aplicó el Cuestionario de conocimiento sobre la esquizofrenia y un instrumento para recolectar información sociodemográfica. Los participantes fueron reclutados en lugares de gran afluencia de público. Se realizaron análisis de correspondencias múltiples no paramétricos bivariables y de conglomerados jerárquicos.

Resultados

El cuestionario presenta dos dimensiones: «Creencias sobre el conocimiento de la esquizofrenia» y «Actitudes hacia la esquizofrenia»; existen diferencias significativas entre estas y el contacto con personas con un trastorno mental grave. En el análisis por conglomerados, se encontraron 3 grupos diferenciados en función de la combinación de las 2 dimensiones del instrumento.

Conclusiones

Destaca que ninguna de las 2 dimensiones mide conocimiento veraz, y el cuestionario tiene una dimensión actitudinal. Más que el contacto en sí, es el tipo de interacción una variable relevante en el nivel de estigma, lo que cuestiona la hipótesis tradicional del contacto. Se requiere investigar mejor las características del instrumento y los aspectos del contacto que se asocian a un menor nivel de estigma en la población.

Palabras clave:
Estigma social
Esquizofrenia
Opinión pública
Actitudes
Texto completo
Introduction

Stigma can be considered as the principal source of suffering for people diagnosed with severe mental illnesses (SMI), mainly schizophrenia, because of the significant psychosocial impact it generates.1,2 Stigma refers to the process of labelling, loss of status and discrimination a person is subject to when they have a characteristic that the social group belittles.3 Among the main consequences of stigma are the deterioration in self-image,4 difficulties in access to employment, housing and social relationships,5 and the barrier effect it generates in health services.6

Stigma is a complex phenomenon with different degrees; researchers distinguish between structural stigma, public stigma and self-stigma.7

In public stigma, the general population ascribes negative stereotypes to a social group based on prejudice, which translates into discriminatory behaviour.8 People who are diagnosed with schizophrenia are often seen as violent, dangerous, unpredictable and not competent to make their own decisions.9,10 A number of different variables have been found to influence peoples’ attitudes. They include age, educational level and previous contact with somebody with SMI.11 Although in Latin America, and particularly in Chile, the subject has been little studied,12,13 research results are consistent with those found in other parts of the world; there are prejudices and negative attitudes towards people with schizophrenia.14,15

Public stigma has been studied through the beliefs and attitudes of the population towards people with a psychiatric diagnosis.16 Different instruments have been used to evaluate the stigma, including the social distance scales, the semantic differential scale and attitude and knowledge scales.17,18 Some studies show a relationship between the level of information a person has about a psychiatric diagnosis and their attitudes towards that population. This seems to be a dialectical relationship, as it has been found that prejudices towards a certain social group influence the degree and type of information people absorb about that group.18

Information can reduce prejudices, both directly through the questioning of myths, and indirectly by increasing awareness of these people's situation. The information is essentially assessed using quick-to-complete true/false and multiple-choice questionnaires. Measures such as these have the advantage of being relatively easy to apply and score, and they are sensitive to change, so they are used in anti-stigma programmes and reduce social desirability.18

Of these instruments, the information scale used by the World Psychiatric Association to evaluate the educational components of its “open the doors” programme is one of the most widely used.10,19 This measurement tool was adapted and used in a research study in Argentina.13

The few studies to have evaluated stigma in Chile have mainly used attitude scales and instruments of their own. It would therefore be useful to have other measures endorsed in different cultural contexts; particularly if we consider that stigma is a public health problem, as it directly affects the chances of recovery and, consequently, of social inclusion of individuals with severe psychiatric illnesses such as schizophrenia.

The aim of this study was to assess public stigma by measuring knowledge and relating the level of information with variables which, in other cultural contexts, have been shown to influence the stigma the population attaches to people with schizophrenia.

Material and methods

We conducted a descriptive, cross-sectional study using surveys.

Participants

A convenience sample was taken of the general population in the province of Concepción in Chile. The only criterion for inclusion was to be aged 18–65. The sample consisted of 399 people, with a mean age of 39.14±12.56 years (range 18–65). In terms of level of education, 53% had higher education (technical or university); 38% secondary education, and just 8.8% had primary education only. There were no subjects with no formal education.

Other socio-demographic characteristics and background and knowledge about schizophrenia are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Socio-demographic data for the sample.

Subjects, n  399 
Females  55.4% 
Occupation
Student  11.5% 
Retired  4.3% 
Unemployed  4.0% 
Housewife  11.0% 
Employed  69.2% 
Have you been diagnosed with a mental illness? Yes  6.3% 
Do you have a relative who suffers from a severe mental illness, for example schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, psychosis, etc.? Yes  19.0% 
Do you have contact with that relative? Yes  69.7% (n=76) 
Do you have a neighbour who suffers from a severe mental illness, for example schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, psychosis, etc.? Yes  25.6% (n=102) 
Do you keep in contact with a neighbour or somebody you know with a severe mental illness, for example schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, psychosis, etc.? Yes  69.3% (n=102) 
Number of members in your family
1–3  31.8% 
4–6  58.4% 
7 or more  9.8% 
InstrumentsKnowledge about schizophrenia questionnaire

Instrument used by the WHO in the implementation of the “open the doors” programme.1,19 In this study, we used the version adapted in Argentina,13 which consists of ten items: eight items have a yes/no/don’t know answer format; the ninth item, which investigates the causes of schizophrenia, gives the multiple choice options: genetic, environmental or both; and the last item, on knowledge about more effective treatments, gives the options: medication, psychotherapy or both.

We added an additional item which asked the question, “does medical treatment help them to remain stable?”, with the choice being yes/no/don’t know. This question would allow us to evaluate the information people have about the relationship between the illness and medical care, which (in the Chilean context) is relevant for assessing the consolidation of a community mental health model (Table 2). The instrument provides a total score of up to 11 points.

Table 2.

Item response rates.

Knowledge about people with schizophreniaYes, %  No, %  Don’t know, % 
They have a split or multiple personality  58.6  11.0a  30.3 
They hear voices that aren’t there  79.4a  4.3  16.3 
They tend to be socially isolated  67.7a  12.0  20.3 
They may suffer from apathy  54.1a  15.5  30.3 
They are mentally retarded  11.5  69.2a  19.3 
The majority are a danger to others or behave violently  56.1  25.1a  18.8 
They behave strangely or inappropriately  79.4a  8.0  12.5 
The majority can be cured  41.6  20.6a  37.8 
Healthcare treatment means they can be kept stable  83.0a  3.8  13.3 
10  The causes of schizophrenia are...  Genetic 43.1  Environmental 7.3  Both 49.6a 
11  The most appropriate treatment is...  Medication 12.8  Psychotherapy 7.8  Both 79.4a 
a

Correct answer.

Socio-demographic data collection questionnaire

This included questions about age, gender, marital status, level of education, employment status and number of members in the participant's family. It also asked participants if they had ever been diagnosed with a mental illness. Lastly, they were asked about whether or not they had a relative or neighbour with a severe mental illness and whether or not they had contact with them.

Procedure

The participants were recruited in places with large gatherings of people: bus terminals, town councils, health-centre waiting rooms, town squares, universities and supermarkets, and in different districts in the selected area.

The instruments were applied by specially trained psychology students. The people who agreed to participate signed an informed consent form which stated the nature of the study and the ethical safeguards in place. In the majority of cases, the questionnaires were self-applied. If a subject had difficulty answering the questions, an interviewer would help them. The data was collected from November to December 2011.

Data analysis

The first review of the database showed <5% missing data in each item, for which a multiple imputation method was applied. A descriptive analysis was then carried out on the socio-demographic variables and, based on the response distributions, an internal consistency analysis was performed. Since this was low, we proceeded to review the structure of the questionnaire using a multiple correspondence analysis, which yielded two dimensions in the instrument.

As the distribution of the data did not conform to a normal curve, non-parametric analyses were used. Bivariate analyses were performed between the dimensions and the socio-demographic variables using the Mann–Whitney U test for dichotomous variables, and the Kruskal–Wallis test for polytomous variables.

Lastly, we analysed hierarchical clusters using the scores for the dimensions in order to classify the participants. We used the Ward metric as distance index, which helped maximise the homogeneity within the groups. We used full grouping as the grouping method. The SPSS version 19.0 software program was used to analyse the results.

Results

A higher score on the knowledge scale indicates more information about schizophrenia. The average score for correct answers was 6.19±1.898, which is equivalent to 56.2%; the median was 6, with an asymmetry of −0.441; 8.8% of the sample obtained scores from 0 to 3, 63.9% from 4 to 7, and 27.3% over 7. Detailed analysis of the answers showed that 58.6% think that people with schizophrenia have a split personality; 56.1% see them as dangerous and 43.1% believe that the causes are purely genetic (Table 2).

To analyse the structure of the scale, we performed a multiple correspondence analysis. A 2-dimensional model was adapted to the data. The first dimension was called “beliefs on the knowledge about schizophrenia” and considered two categories. The yes/no answers tended to be grouped as one category and the “don’t know” answers as another. These two categories were scored with values 1 and 0, respectively. Higher values in the dimension would indicate that the respondent had a stronger belief in their knowledge about schizophrenia, reflected in fewer “don’t know” answers.

The second dimension referred to peoples’ attitudes based on that knowledge. Negative attitudes were associated with high scores and positive attitudes with low scores. However, there was one group that obtained intermediate scores, so the answers were scored 0, 1 and 2, respectively. This dimension was called “attitudes towards schizophrenia” and was covered by items 1–9. Items 10 (“causes of schizophrenia”) and 11 (“the most appropriate treatment is...”), however, did not seem to be associated with any dimension, so they were excluded from the following analyses.

The analysis of internal consistency of dimension 1 (“beliefs on the knowledge about schizophrenia”) produced a Cronbach's alpha of 0.721; while dimension 2 (“attitudes towards schizophrenia”) produced α=0.523.

The bivariate analyses between the dimensions and the socio-demographic variables showed no significant differences in dimension 1 according to gender, level of education, age, family income, family size or marital status. Significant differences were found when the person interviewed stated that they had been diagnosed with a severe mental illness (SMI) (U=2.952, p=0.003), had a family member with SMI (U=4.352; p<0.001), or knew a neighbour with SMI (U=3.061, p=0.002). In these situations, there was a stronger belief in the level of knowledge about schizophrenia. In the case of occupation, although there was a significant difference between the different categories of the variable, it was not possible to determine between which it occurred (Table 3).

Table 3.

Results by dimensions. Dimension 1. Trend of knowledge about severe mental illnesses.

Variable  Mean±SD  Median (95% CI)  p 
Gender
Males  6.89±2.112  7.0 (6.58–7.20)  0.375 
Females  7.11±1.953  8.0 (6.85–7.37)   
Education
Primary  7.34±1.846  8.0 (6.71–7.98)  0.084 
Secondary  6.88±2.151  7.5 (6.53–7.22)   
Higher technical  6.69±2.095  7.0 (6.26–7.12)   
University  7.34±1.808  8.0 (7.01–7.67)   
Age
18–29  6.81±2.162  7.0 (6.41–7.22)  0.296 
30–41  7.05±2.024  8.0 (6.66–7.43)   
42–53  6.97±1.982  7.0 (6.60–7.33)   
54–65  7.39±1.837  8.0 (6.92–7.86)   
Occupation
Student  6.28±2.437  7.0 (5.56–7.01)  0.045a 
Retired  7.65±2.149  9.0 (6.54–8.75)   
Unemployed  7.56±2.065  8.5 (6.46–8.66)   
Housewife  6.89±2.233  7.0 (6.21–7.57)   
Employed  7.08±1.884  8.0 (6.86–7.30)   
Family income ($)
<300,000  6.95±2.067  7.5 (6.63–7.27)  0.756 
300,001–500,000  6.96±2.087  7.0 (6.54–7.38)   
≥500,001  7.11±1.947  8.0 (6.79–7.43)   
Number of members in the family
1–3  7.20±1.891  8.0 (6.87–7.54)  0.577 
4–6  6.91±2.105  7.0 (6.64–7.19)   
≥7  6.95±1.973  8.0 (6.31–7.59)   
Person diagnosed with mental illness
No  6.94±2.052  7.0 (6.73–7.14)  0.003b 
Yes  8.12±1.130  8.0 (7.65–8.59)   
Relative with mental illness
No  6.81±2.074  7.0 (6.58–7.04)  <0.001b 
Yes  7.86±1.555  8.0 (7.50–8.21)   
Contact with relative
No  7.57±2.085  8.0 (6.66–8.47)  0.743 
Yes  7.83±1.673  8.0 (7.37–8.29)   
Neighbour with mental illness
No  6.79±2.169  7.0 (6.54–7.04)  0.002b 
Yes  7.65±1.354  8.0 (7.38–7.91)   
Contact with neighbour
No  7.65±1.355  8.0 (7.15–8.14)  0.927 
Yes  7.66±1.371  8.0 (7.33–7.98)   
Marital status
Single  6.94±2.189  8.0 (6.59–7.29)  0.558 
Married or co-habiting  7.01±1.895  7.0 (6.76–7.27)   
Separated, widowed or divorced  7.26±2.075  8.0 (6.58–7.95)   

SD: standard deviation; 95% CI: 95% confidence interval.

a

p0.05.

b

p0.01.

The analysis of dimension 2 (“attitudes towards schizophrenia”) did not show significant differences with gender, age, family income, having a relative with SMI, knowing a neighbour with SMI or the marital status of the respondents. There were significant differences with the size of the family group, education, having contact with a neighbour with SMI and occupation. Large families had a more favourable attitude than medium-sized families (H=7.260, p=0.028); and people with university education had a more favourable attitude than those with only secondary studies (H=9.788, p=0.016). A more positive attitude was also observed among people who had contact with a neighbour diagnosed with an SMI (U=2.056, p=0.040). Lastly, as in dimension 1, it was not possible to determine the trend of the significant difference in the occupation variable (Table 4). However, these results should be interpreted with caution, since the internal consistency of the dimension is poor (Table 4).

Table 4.

Results by dimensions. Dimension 2. Trend of attitude assessment on severe mental illnesses.

Variables  Mean±SD  Median (95% CI)  p 
Gender
Males  13.46±2.493  14.0 (13.09–13.82)  0.335 
Females  13.53±2.986  14.0 (13.13–13.93)   
Education
Primary  13.83±2.717  14.0 (12.90–14.76)  0.020a 
Secondary  13.76±3.053  14.0 (13.27–14.25)   
Higher technical  13.55±2.547  14.0 (13.03–14.07)   
University  13.01±2.544  14.0 (12.54–13.47)   
Age
18–29  13.48±2.630  14.0 (12.99–13.97)  0.294 
30–41  13.18±3.228  14.0 (12.57–13.79)   
42–53  14.0±2.482  14.0 (13.54–14.46)   
54–65  13.15±2.574  14.0 (12.49–13.81)   
Occupation
Student  13.91±2.336  14.0 (13.22–14.61)  0.049a 
Retired  12.29±3.077  14.0 (10.71–13.88)   
Unemployed  13.25±3.066  13.5 (11.62–14.88)   
Housewife  14.32±3.010  14.5 (13.40–15.23)   
Employed  13.38±2.742  14.0 (13.06–13.71)   
Family income ($)
<300,000  13.62±3.001  14.0 (13.15–14.09)  0.255 
300,001–500,000  13.57±2.440  14.0 (13.08–14.07)   
≥500,001  13.31±2.728  14.0 (12.86–13.76)   
Number of members in the family
1–3  13.35±2.739  14.0 (12.87–13.84)  0.028a 
4–6  13.76±2.670  14.0 (13.42–14.11)   
≥7  12.36±3.216  13.0 (11.32–13.40)   
Person diagnosed with mental illness
No  13.51±2.763  14.0 (13.23–13.79)  0.614 
Yes  13.24±2.976  14.0 (12.01–14.47)   
Relative with mental illness
No  13.55±2.755  14.0 (13.25–13.85)  0.441 
Yes  13.26±2.858  14.0 (12.61–13.92)   
Contact with relative with SMI
No  13.39±3.201  14.0 (12.01–14.78)  0.94 
Yes  13.28±2.804  14.0 (12.51–14.06)   
Neighbour with mental illness
No  13.53±2.865  14.0 (13.20–13.85)  0.415 
Yes  13.41±2.499  14.0 (12.92–13.90)   
Contact with neighbour with SMI
No  14.06±2.435  14.0 (13.17–14.96)  0.040a 
Yes  13.17±2.479  14.0 (12.58–13.76)   
Marital status
Single  13.42±2.480  14.0 (13.02–13.81)  0.465 
Married/Co-habiting  13.50±2.994  14.0 (13.10–13.91)   
Separated, widowed or divorced  13.76±2.686  14.0 (12.88–14.65)   

SD: standard deviation; 95% CI: 95% confidence interval; SMI: severe mental illness.

a

p0.05.

bp0.01.

The hierarchical cluster analysis identified three groups (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Distribution of subjects according to hierarchical cluster analysis.

(0.12MB).

The first group (n=53) corresponded to those with a favourable attitude towards schizophrenia and a stronger belief in knowledge, and was composed mainly of women (66%) and people who were working (74%); 49% lived as a couple. Worth noting is that 36% had a university education and 40% were in the highest income bracket. This group had experience from close contact with mental illness, either because of their own diagnosis (9%) or that of relatives (28%) or neighbours (25%). Consistent with the above, this was the group that had the greatest amount of contact with neighbours who suffered from SMI (92%).

The second group had a negative attitude towards schizophrenia and little belief in knowledge (n=231). This group had a similar number of males to females, although the ratio was slightly biased towards females (54.5%). Like the first group, the majority lived as a couple (52%) and were working (70%); 32% had a university education and 42% came under the lowest income bracket. Lastly, they had less experience with people with SMI than the first group, with 8% reporting a personal diagnosis and 23% in family members. This group had the highest percentage of neighbours with SMI, but had the least amount of contact with them (68%).

The third group (n=115) had a negative attitude towards schizophrenia and a stronger belief in knowledge. In this group, there were similar numbers of males and females (52.2%) and the majority lived as a couple (55%) and were working (66%), although the percentage in employment was lower than in the other two groups. There was a high proportion of people with only secondary education (45%) and the lowest proportion of the three groups with university education (21%). They had less contact with people with SMI, either because few of them had been diagnosed with a psychiatric illness themselves (2%) or had family members (7%) or neighbours (12%) who had (Table 5).

Table 5.

Distribution of socio-demographic variables in the 3 groups, by gender.

  Group 1Group 2Group 3
  Females  Males  All  Females  Males  All  Females  Males  All 
Subjects, n  35  18  53  126  105  231  60  55  115 
Age (years), mean  37.7  41.7  39.1  39.7  39.4  39.6  36.1  40.5  38.2 
Marital status, %
Single  46  33  42  37  39  38  35  40  37 
Married/Co-habiting  43  61  49  49  54  52  52  58  55 
Separated, divorced or widowed  11  13  10  13 
Occupation
Student  11  10  10  10  20  11  16 
Retired 
Unemployed  11 
Housewife  14  20  11  23  12 
Employed  74  72  74  63  78  70  57  76  66 
Schooling
Primary  13  10 
Secondary  34  39  36  40  29  35  55  35  45 
Higher technical  26  11  21  17  28  22  23  33  28 
University  31  44  36  29  36  32  17  25  21 
Family income ($)
<300,000  34  33  34  46  36  42  40  40  40 
300,000–499,000  26  28  26  26  18  23  35  16  26 
≥500,000  40  39  40  28  46  36  25  44  34 
Number of members in the family
1–3  26  28  26  33  39  35  37  16  27 
4–6  49  56  51  60  57  59  55  67  61 
≥7  26  17  23  16  12 
You have been diagnosed with a mental illness
No  91  89  91  90  94  92  97  100  98 
Yes  11  10 
Relative with mental illness
No  71  72  72  75  80  77  92  95  93 
Yes  29  28  28  25  20  23 
Have contact with relative
No  40  27  23  38  29  50  33  44 
Yes  60  100  73  77  62  71  50  67  56 
Neighbour with mental illness
No  71  83  75  67  70  68  88  87  88 
Yes  29  17  25  33  30  32  12  13  12 
Have contact with neighbour
No  10  31  34  32  57  29  43 
Yes  90  100  92  69  66  68  43  71  57 

The values are expressed as percentages unless otherwise indicated.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to assess public stigma by measuring knowledge and relating the level of information with variables which, in other cultural contexts, have been shown to influence the stigma the population attaches to people with schizophrenia.1,2,19

In terms of level of information, 56.2% of people had at least six correct answers out of a maximum of 11 questions. That percentage was lower than those obtained in studies conducted in North America and Europe,1,2,10,19,20 but similar to the aforementioned result in Argentina reported by Leiderman et al.,13 possibly because of the similarity between the two instruments, the method of application (self-applied), and the similar social contexts, as Chile and Argentina are neighbouring countries that share language and customs. The low level of information the population has about schizophrenia may also be associated with the fact that community-orientated intervention models are less well developed.21

Analysing the results by items, 58.6% of the interviewees still thought that people with schizophrenia have a split personality. This result is in line with previous studies in which it has been observed that this condition is associated with schizophrenia,1,2,13,19,22 which feeds myths and prejudices about this disorder.

The study also confirmed the association made by the general population between schizophrenia and danger. Although some studies report that there is no significant difference between people with and without schizophrenia in terms of being violent, and it has even been found that subjects with SMI are more likely to be victims of violence than the general population,23 the public still considers them to be a threat.20 Negative descriptions or generalisations made by the media about violent events contribute to the construction of beliefs, prejudices and, ultimately, public stigma.24–26

As found in other studies,1,2,19 a high percentage of people (43.1%) thought that genetics were the sole cause of schizophrenia, which shows that the psychosocial aspects associated with the onset of the disorder are still unknown to a significant part of the population. However, in contrast to the above, 79% of respondents believed the best treatment to be a combination of medication and psychotherapy and 41.6% stated that the majority of people with schizophrenia could be cured. Those findings show the contradictory nature of the information people have and reflect conflicting knowledge that can contribute to maintaining ambivalent attitudes towards people with schizophrenia. This more positive view could be related to the fact that for some decades now in Chile, a community mental health model has been used to address SMI.27,28

As far as the structure of the scale is concerned, it is interesting that an instrument that evaluates information has two dimensions, neither of which is based on its degree of veracity. The first dimension refers to the beliefs people have about their knowledge on the subject. These beliefs are related to direct contact with people suffering from an SMI, which could mean that direct experience influences people's ideas about their level of information. In the study conducted by Leiderman et al.13 in Argentina, contact was associated with the level of true information. Therefore, it is plausible to conclude that this variable is associated both with real knowledge and with the knowledge beliefs that people have about schizophrenia. This is an interesting idea, because it suggests that for some people, contact may affect the level of information, while in others, it only affects the belief at that level. That then leads to the question of whether actual experience with people with SMI can reinforce existing stereotypes, as people adapt new information to their beliefs through bias.29 However, we need to identify what factors contribute to this process.

Another unusual aspect is for a knowledge scale to have a dimension that reflects attitudes. As various authors have pointed out, assessing the level of information about SMI is an indirect way of measuring attitudes. This idea supports the use of information questionnaires as attitudinal measures, with the advantage of having less social desirability bias.18

Contact with neighbours with SMI is associated with a more positive attitude, which reaffirms the importance of contact, but shows that the type of relationship established is central. When the interaction is less intimate, the attitude is more favourable, probably because the contact is less charged with negative emotions. Studies have found that relatives of people with schizophrenia have a more negative attitude towards the disease than other social groups, a result that contrasts with the traditional contact hypothesis.30

Families with more members, however, have a more positive attitude towards schizophrenia. We were unable to find any studies that directly relate this variable to attitudes, but, in view of this finding, we need to establish more precisely how many people in the family group make the difference and what characteristics these family groups possess. Lastly, our study corroborates the fact that people with a higher level of education have a more favourable attitude towards SMI.31–33

In the cluster analysis, the group with the strongest belief in knowledge and a more positive attitude was composed mainly of women, had a higher level of education and income and had direct contact with people with SMI. This is in line with the findings of other studies, in which women had a more positive attitude towards SMI, and may be explained by the fact that women are more concerned with issues and aspects related to healthcare.1,19

The second group identified in the cluster analysis had little belief in knowledge about schizophrenia and a negative attitude. This group, like the first, had contact with people with SMI. However, when comparing them, we found that, while interaction with relatives was similar (73% versus 71%), the group with favourable attitudes had more contact with neighbours (92% versus 68%). This reaffirms what we stated earlier, that it is not the contact itself that makes the difference, it is the type of contact that could be affecting attitudes towards people with schizophrenia. Future research should investigate what the differences are between contact with relatives and contact with neighbours that facilitate the acquisition of positive attitudes towards people with SMI.

Last of all, the third group had a strong belief in knowledge and a negative attitude towards SMI. In this case, the ideas people have about the information they have lead them to justify or “rationalise” their position on the subject34; the belief in knowledge, even if wrong, gives basis to the negative attitude, to the extent that an explanation about SMI is given. People therefore reacted to the items according to their general attitude and selected the information consistent with that attitude.35 In social psychology, maintaining consistency and avoiding disparate information in such a way is termed congeniality bias.36,37 This group was the one that presented the biggest challenge in the intervention because, as they thought they knew about the subject, they were less willing to receive or seek alternative information that questioned their attitudes.

One of the limitations of the study was the non-probability sampling, which means we have to be careful about generalising the results. Furthermore, the scale used did not reflect the knowledge well, and further research therefore needs to be carried out on the structure of the scale. Lastly, further research is also necessary to take a closer and more in-depth look at how stigma develops towards people with schizophrenia among the population in Chile. In view of the relevance of psychosocial aspects in these people's recovery, that information is important for the future development of interventions and care. Moreover, there has been little research in this area in Chile. Therefore, this type of study will increase knowledge on the subject, not only in Chile but also in Latin America, and allow us to measure the true scope of the stigma towards SMI.

Conclusions

The population has a poor level of information and stereotypical beliefs about schizophrenia. The questionnaire on knowledge about schizophrenia had two dimensions: “beliefs on the knowledge of schizophrenia” and “attitudes towards schizophrenia”. It is important, on the one hand, that neither dimension measured true knowledge and, on the other, that a knowledge scale can evaluate attitudes, supporting the use of information questionnaires as attitudinal measures. The results of this study reinforce the importance of contact, but raise questions about the traditional hypothesis as, more than contact itself, it is the type of contact that becomes a significant variable in the level of stigma. Further research is required on the characteristics of the tool and the aspects of the contact associated to a lower level of stigma in the population.

Ethical disclosuresProtection of human and animal subjects

The authors declare that the procedures followed were in accordance with the regulations of the relevant clinical research ethics committee and with those of the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

Confidentiality of data

The authors declare that no patient data appear in this article.

Right to privacy and informed consent

The authors declare that no patient data appear in this article.

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References
[1]
H. Stuart, J. Arboleda-Flórez.
Community attitudes toward people with schizophrenia.
Can J Psychiatry, 46 (2001), pp. 245-252
[2]
J. Arboleda-Flórez.
Stigma and discrimination: an overview.
World Psychiatry, 4 (2005), pp. 8-10
[3]
B.G. Link, J. Phelan.
Conceptualizing stigma.
Ann Rev Soc, 27 (2001), pp. 363-385
[4]
M.V. Karidi, C.N. Stefanis, C. Theleritis, M. Tzedaki, A.D. Rabavilas, N.C. Stefanis.
Perceived social stigma, self-concept, and self-stigmatization of patient with schizophrenia.
Comprehen Psychiatry, 51 (2010), pp. 19-30
[5]
J. Sharac, P. Mccrone, S. Clement, G. Thornicroft.
The economic impact of mental health stigma and discrimination: a sistematic review.
Int J Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci, 19 (2010), pp. 223-232
[6]
O. Wahl.
Stigma as a barrier to recovery from mental illness.
Trend Cogn Sci, 16 (2012), pp. 9-10
[7]
P.W. Corrigan, J.R. O'Shaughnessy.
Changing mental illness stigma as it exists in the real world.
Aust Psychol, 42 (2007), pp. 90-97
[8]
S. Hinshaw.
Stigma and mental illness: developmental issues and future prospects.
Developmental psychopathology, pp. 841-881
[9]
D. Rose, G. Thornicroft, V. Pinfold, A. Kassam.
250 labels used to stigmatise people with mental illness.
Health Serv Res, 7 (2007), pp. 7-97
[10]
A.H. Thompson, H. Stuart, R.C. Bland, J. Arboleda-Flórez, R. Warner, R.A. Dickson.
Attitudes about schizophrenia from the pilot site of the WPA worldwide campaign against the stigma of schizophrenia.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 37 (2002), pp. 475-482
[11]
M.C. Angermeyer, S. Dietrich.
Public beliefs about and attitudes towards people with mental illness: a review of population studies.
Acta Psychiatr Scand, 113 (2006), pp. 163-179
[12]
E. Toledo, S.L. Blay.
Community perception of mental disorders. A systematic review of Latin American and Caribbean studies.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 39 (2004), pp. 955-961
[13]
E.A. Leiderman, G. Vazquez, C. Berizzo, A. Bonifacio, N. Bruscoli, J. Capria, et al.
Public knowledge, beliefs and attitudes towards patients with schizophrenia: Buenos Aires.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 46 (2011), pp. 281-290
[14]
J. Chuaqui, Esquizofrenia.
estigma e inserción laboral.
Psiquiatría y Salud Mental, 1 (2002), pp. 4-11
[15]
C. Zárate, M. Ceballos, M. Contardo, R. Florenzano.
Influencia de dos factores en la percepción hacia los enfermos mentales; contacto cercano y educación en salud.
Rev Chil Neuro-Psiquiatr, 44 (2006), pp. 205-214
[16]
A. Jorm, C. Kelly.
Improving the public's understanding and response to mental disorders.
Aust Psychol, 42 (2007), pp. 81-89
[17]
B. Link, L. Yang, J. Phelan, P. Collins.
Measuring mental illness stigma.
Schizophr Bull, 30 (2004), pp. 511-541
[18]
P.W. Corrigan, J.R. Shapiro.
Measuring the impact of programs that challenge the public stigma of mental illness.
Clin Psychol Rev, 30 (2010), pp. 907-922
[19]
W. Gaebel, A. Baumann, A. Witte, H. Zaeske.
Public attitudes towards people with mental illness in six German cities.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 252 (2002), pp. 278-287
[20]
Ruiz MÁ, J.M. Montes, J.C. Lauffer, C. Álvarez, J. Maurino, C. De Dios Perrino.
Opiniones y creencias sobre las enfermedades mentales graves (esquizofrenia y trastorno bipolar) en la sociedad española.
Psiquiatría y Salud Mental, 5 (2012), pp. 98-106
[21]
J. Caldas de Almeida.
Estrategias de cooperación técnica de la Organización Panamericana de la Salud en la nueva fase de la reforma de los servicios de salud mental en América Latina y el Caribe.
Rev Panam Salud Publ, 18 (2005), pp. 314-326
[22]
G. Schomerus, D. Kenzin, J. Borsche, H. Matschinger, M.C. Angermeyer.
The association of schizophrenia with split personality is not an ubiquitous phenomenon.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 42 (2007), pp. 780-786
[23]
N.J. Reavley, A.F. Jorm, A.J. Morgan.
Beliefs about dangerousness of people with mental health problems: the role of media reports and personal exposure to threat or harm.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 50 (2016), pp. 1-8
[24]
A.M. Möller-Leimkühler, H.J. Möller, W. Maier, W. Gaebel, P. Falkai.
EPA guidance on improving the image of psychiatry.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 266 (2006), pp. 139-154
[25]
Y. Aoki, E. Malcolm, S. Yamaguchi, G. Thornicroft, C. Henderson.
Mental illness among journalists: a systematic review.
Int J Soc Psychiatry, 59 (2013), pp. 377-390
[26]
P.J. Michaels, P.W. Corrigan.
Measuring mental illness stigma with diminished social desirability effects.
J Mental Health, 22 (2013), pp. 218-226
[27]
A. Minoletti, P. Narváez, R. Sepúlveda, A. Caprile.
Chile: Lecciones aprendidas en la implementación de un modelo comunitario de atención en salud mental Chile.
Salud mental en la comunidad, pp. 339-348
[28]
Plan Nacional de Salud Mental y Psiquiatría.
Ministerio de Salud (MINSAL), (2001),
[29]
M. Barreto.
Experiencing and coping with social stigma.
APA handbook of personality and social psychology: vol II. Group processes, pp. 473-506
[30]
D. Rose, R. Willis, E. Brohan, N. Sartorius, C. Villares, K. Wahlbeck, et al.
Reported stigma and discrimination by people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia.
Epidemiol Psych Sci, 20 (2011), pp. 193-204
[31]
P.W. Corrigan, A.C. Watson.
The stigma of psychiatric disorders and the gender, ethnicity, and education of the perceiver.
Commun Ment Health J, 43 (2007), pp. 439-458
[32]
E. Girma, A.M. Möller-Leimkühler, N. Müller, S. Dehning, G. Froeschl, M. Tesfaye.
Public stigma against family members of people with mental illness: findings from the Gilgel Gibe Field Research Center (GGFRC), Southwest Ethiopia.
BMC Int Health Hum Rights, 14 (2014), pp. 1
[33]
J.P. Stuber, A. Rocha, A. Christian, B.G. Link.
Conceptions of mental illness: attitudes of mental health professionals and the general public.
Psychiatr Serv, 65 (2014), pp. 490-497
[34]
J.F. Valencia, L. Gil-De-Montes, F. Elejabarrieta.
Creencias y actitudes hacia la inmigración: estereotipos, prejuicio y regulaciones normativas.
Int J Soc Psychol, 19 (2004), pp. 299-318
[35]
A. Molero.
El estudio del prejuicio en psicología social: definición y causas.
Psicología social, pp. 591-616
[36]
A. Eagly, S. Chaiken.
Attitude research in the 21st century: the current state of knowledge.
The hanbook of attitudes, pp. 743-767
[37]
S. Oskamp, P.W. Schultz.
Attitudes and opinions.
3rd ed., Psychology Press, (2005),

Please cite this article as: Grandón P, Aguilera AV, Bustos C, Alzate EC, Saldivia S. Evaluación del estigma hacia personas con diagnóstico de esquizofrenia mediante una escala de conocimiento. Rev Colomb Psiquiat. 2018;47:72–81.

Copyright © 2017. Asociación Colombiana de Psiquiatría
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo