metricas
covid
Buscar en
Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría
Toda la web
Inicio Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría Dimensiones de la empatía en excombatientes del conflicto armado colombiano uti...
Información de la revista
Vol. 42. Núm. 1.
Páginas 9-28 (marzo 2013)
Compartir
Compartir
Descargar PDF
Más opciones de artículo
Vol. 42. Núm. 1.
Páginas 9-28 (marzo 2013)
Artículos originales
Acceso a texto completo
Dimensiones de la empatía en excombatientes del conflicto armado colombiano utilizando una escala estandarizada*
Dimensions of Empathy in Ex-Combatants of the Colombian Armed Conflict Using a Standardized Scale
Visitas
1077
David A. Pineda1,
Autor para correspondencia
dpineda12@hotmail.com

Correspondencia: David A. Pineda, Sede de Investigaciones Universitarias (SIU), Calle 62 No. 52-59, laboratorio 412, Medellín, Colombia
, Daniel Camilo Aguirre-Acevedo2, Natalia Trujillo3, Ana María Valencia4, Ángela Pareja5, Carlos Tobón6, Lina Velilla7, Agustín Ibáñez8
1 Médico neurólogo, Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia. Grupo de Neuropsicología y Conducta, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
2 Mg. Epidemiología PhDc, Grupo de Neuropsicología y Conducta, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
3 Psicóloga PhD, Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia. Grupo de Neuropsicología y Conducta, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
4 Psicóloga, Grupo Neuropsicología y Conducta, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
5 Psicóloga, Grupo de Neuropsicología y Conducta, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
6 Médico, PhDc, Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia. Grupo de Neuropsicología y Conducta, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
7 Psicóloga, Grupo de Neurociencias de Antioquia, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
8 Psicólogo PhD, Instituto de Neurología Cognitiva (INECO), Universidad Favaloro, Buenos Aires, Argentina, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas (CONICET), Buenos Aires, Argentina, Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Introducción

La empatía es uno de los constructos de la llamadas neurociencias sociales. Se define como una disposición de dimensiones múltiples que permite colocarse en el estado emocional del otro. Colombia presenta un conflicto interno irregular prolongado, que ha venido deteriorándose en crueldad.

Objetivos

Evaluar las dimensiones de la empatía de 285 excombatientes del conflicto armado colombiano, usando el Índice de Reactividad Interpersonal (IRI) en español.

Métodos y sujetos

Se seleccionó una muestra de 285 excombatientes, 241 (84,6%) hombres: 85,3% paramilitares y 14,7% guerrilleros. Se les aplicó el IRI de 28 ítems. Se realizaron 3 análisis de factores exploratorios (AFE) y análisis factoriales confirmatorios (AFC), utilizando ecuaciones estructurales.

Resultados

Un primer AFE obtuvo 9 factores (KMO = 0,74 y 54,7% de varianza, consistencia interna [CI]: 0,22-0,63). Un segundo AFE con 20 ítems con cargas superiores a 0,4 mostró una estructura de 6 factores (KMO = 0,70, 50,3% de varianza; CI: 0,37-0,63). Un tercer AFE forzó las 4 dimensiones originales (KMO = 0,74, varianza 33,77% y CI: 0,44-0,77). El AFC mostró índices de bondad de ajuste adecuados para los 3 modelos. El de 4 factores obtuvo los más bajos, y el de 6 factores, los más altos. El modelo de 4 factores obtuvo la mejor CI.

Conclusión

El IRI en español en excombatientes del conflicto colombiano tiene estructuras posibles de 4, 6 y 9 factores. El mejor ajuste es la de 6 factores. La de 4 factores tuvo mejor CI.

Palabras clave:
Empatía
neurociencias sociales
cognición social
análisis factorial
violencia
Abstract
Introduction

Empathy is one of the main concepts of in social neurosciences. It is defined as a trait with multiple dimensions allowing individuals to place themselves in the emotional states of others. Colombia has an irregular, internal and long-lasting armed conflict which has been increasing its cruelty levels.

Objectives

to assess the empathy dimensions of 285 ex-combatants from the internal Colombian conflict, using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index(IRI) in Spanish.

Methodology and Subjects

a sample of 285 male ex-combatants, 241 (84, 6%) males: 85,3% paramilitaries, and 14,7% guerillas. The 28 Item IRI questionnaires were administered. 3 exploratory factor analyses (EFA) were performed. Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were developed using structural equation procedures.

Results

The first EFA obtained 9 factors (KMO=0,74, variance 54,7% and internal consistency (IC): 0,22 – 0,63). The second EFA produced 20 items with burdens above 0,4 and showed a 6-factor structure (KMO=0,70, variance 50,3%, IC: 0,37 – 0,63). The third EFA forced the 4 original IRI dimensions (KMO=0,74, variance 33,77, IC: 0,44 – 0,77. CFAs showed goodness of adjustment indexes adequate for the three models. The 4-factor model obtained the lowest value, while the 6-factor model obtained the highest. The 4- factor model showed the best IC.

Conclusion

The Spanish IRI administered to ex-combatants of the Colombian conflict has possible structures of 4, 6 and 9 factors. The best adjustment was for the 6-fctor. The 4-factor model exhibited the best IC.

Key words:
Emphaty
social neurosciences
social cognition
factor analysis
violence
El Texto completo está disponible en PDF
Referencias
[1]
R Dunbar, S Shultz.
Evolution in the Social Brain.
Science, 317 (2007), pp. 1344-1347
[2]
J Decety.
The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans.
Dev Neurosci, 32 (2010), pp. 257-267
[3]
MT Banich, KL Mackiewicz, BE Depue, et al.
Cognitive control mechanisms, emotion and memory: A neural perspective with implications for psychopathology.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 33 (2009), pp. 613-630
[4]
N Eisenberg.
Emotion, regulation, and moral development.
Annu Rev Psychol, 51 (2000), pp. 665-697
[5]
ES Barratt, MS Stanford, L Dowdy, et al.
Impulsive and premeditated aggression: a factor analysis of self-reported acts.
Psychiatry Res, 86 (1999), pp. 163-173
[6]
L Moya-Albiol.
Bases neurales de la violencia humana.
Rev Neurol, 38 (2004), pp. 1067-1075
[7]
MFP De Araújo, E Hori, RS Maiora, et al.
Neuronal activity of the anterior cingulate cortexduring an observation-based decision making task in monkeys.
Behav Brain Res, 230 (2012), pp. 48-61
[8]
G Liakakisa, J Nickela, RJ Seitz.
Diversity of the inferior frontal gyrus: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies.
Behav Brain Res, 225 (2011), pp. 341-347
[9]
T Singer, R Snozzi, G Bird, et al.
Effects of oxytocin and prosocial behavior on brain responses to direct and vicariously experienced pain.
Emotion, 8 (2008), pp. 781-791
[10]
A Azurmendi, F Braza, A García, et al.
Aggression, dominance, and affiliation: their relationships with androgen levels and intelligence in 5-year-old children.
Horm Behav, 50 (2006), pp. 132-140
[11]
Y Cheng, AN Meltzoff, J Decety.
Motivation modulates the activity of the human mirror-neuron system cerebral.
Cortex, 17 (2007), pp. 1979-1986
[12]
PB Pascolo, A Cattarinussi.
On the relationship between mouth opening and “broken mirror neurons” in autistic individuals.
J Electromyogr Kinesiol, 22 (2012), pp. 98-102
[13]
PG Enticott, HA Kennedy, NJ Rinehart, et al.
Mirror neuron activity associated with social impairments but not age in autism spectrum disorder.
Biol Psychiatry, 71 (2012), pp. 427-433
[14]
G Buccino, M Amore.
Mirror neurons and the understanding of behavioural symptoms in psychiatric disorders.
Curr Opin Psychiatry, 21 (2008), pp. 281-285
[15]
J Burns.
The social brain hypothesis of schizophrenia.
World Psychiatry, 5 (2006), pp. 77-81
[16]
Y Cheng, CP Lin, HL Liu, et al.
Expertise modulates the perception of pain in others.
Curr Biol, 17 (2007), pp. 1708-1713
[17]
B Lorey, M Kaletsch, S Pilgramm, et al.
Confidence in emotion perception in point-light displays varies with the ability to perceive own emotions.
PLoS One, 7 (2012), pp. 42169
[18]
J Blos, A Chatterjee, T Kircher, et al.
Neural correlates of causality judgment in physical and social context-The reversed effects of space and time.
Neuroimage, 63 (2012), pp. 882-893
[19]
DC Richardson, CN Street, JY Tan, et al.
Joint perception: gaze and social context.
Front Hum Neurosci, 6 (2012), pp. 194
[20]
A Ibáñez, E Hurtado, A Lobos, et al.
Subliminal presentation of other faces (but not own face) primes behavioral and evoked cortical processing of empathy for pain.
Brain Res, 1398 (2011), pp. 72-85
[21]
D Cosmelli, A Ibáñez.
Human cognition in contex: on the biologic cognitive and social reconsideration of meaning as making sense of action.
Integ Psychol Behav Sci, 42 (2008), pp. 233-244
[22]
A Verdejo-García, A Bechara.
A somatic marker theory of addiction.
Neuropharmacology, 56 (2009), pp. 48-62
[23]
BD Dunn, T Dalgleish, AD Lawrence.
The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 30 (2006), pp. 239-271
[24]
TL Chartrand, JA Bargh.
The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction.
J Pers Soc Psychol, 76 (1999), pp. 893-910
[25]
P Mengotti, C Corradi-Dell'acqua, RI Rumiati.
Imitation components in the human brain: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage, 59 (2012), pp. 1622-1630
[26]
J Decety, J Grèzes.
The power of simulation: imagining one's own and other's behavior.
[27]
J Decety, J Grèzes.
Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions.
Trends Cogn Sci, 3 (1999), pp. 172-178
[28]
L Moya-Albiol, N Herrero, MC Bernal.
Bases neuronales de la empatía.
Rev Neurol, 50 (2010), pp. 89-100
[29]
MH Davis.
A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy.
JSAS, 10 (1980), pp. 85-104
[30]
EL Lev.
Bandura's theory of self-efficacy: applications to oncology.
Sch Inq Nurs Pract, 11 (1997), pp. 21-37
[31]
A Bandura.
Comments on the crusade against the causal efficacy of human thought.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry, 26 (1995), pp. 179-190
[32]
JL Burnette, EH O'Boyle, EM Vanepps, et al.
Mind-Sets matter: a meta-analytic review of implicit theories and self-regulation.
Psychol Bull, (2012),
[33]
I Konvalinka, A Roepstorff.
The two-brain approach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction?.
Front Hum Neurosci, 6 (2012), pp. 215
[34]
L Carr, M Iacoboni, MC Dubeau, et al.
Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 100 (2003), pp. 5497-5502
[35]
J Decety.
Perspective taking as the royal avenue to empathy.
Other minds: How humans bridge the divide between self and other, pp. 135-149
[36]
J Decety, PL Jackson.
The functional architecture of human empathy.
Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev, 3 (2004), pp. 71-100
[37]
C Lamm, CD Batson, J Decety.
The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal.
J Cogn Neurosci, 19 (2007), pp. 42-58
[38]
M Schaefer, H Heinze, M Rotte.
Embodied empathy for tactile events: Interindividual differences and vicarious somatosensory responses during touch observation.
Neuroimage, 60 (2012), pp. 952-957
[39]
Z Kunda.
Social cognition: making sense of people, MIT Press, (1999),
[40]
A Ibáñez, F Manes.
Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia.
Neurology, 78 (2012), pp. 1354-1362
[41]
MH Davis.
Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach.
J Person Soc Psychopathol, 44 (1983), pp. 113-126
[42]
V Mestre-Escrivá, MD Frías-Navarro, P Samper-García.
La medida de la empatía: análisis del Interpersonal Reactivity Index.
Psicothema, 16 (2004), pp. 255-260
[43]
RT Azevedo, E Macaluso, A Avenanti, et al.
Their pain is not our pain: Brain and autonomic correlates of empathic resonance with the pain of same and different race individuals.
Hum Brain Mapp, (2012),
[44]
DM Amodio, HK Hamilton.
Intergroup anxiety effects on implicit racial evaluation and stereotyping.
Emotion, (2012),
[45]
K Gonsalkorale, JW Sherman, TJ Allen, et al.
Accounting for successful control of implicit racial bias: the roles of association activation, response monitoring, and overcoming bias.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull, 37 (2011), pp. 1534-1545
[46]
RH Hoyle.
Structural equation modeling, concepts, issues and applications, Sage Publications, (1995),
[47]
L Hu, PM Bentler.
Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives.
Struct Equat Mod, 6 (1999), pp. 1-55
[48]
RL Gorsuch.
Factor analysis, 2nd ed., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, (1983),
[49]
MA Martínez-González, A Sánchez-Villegas, FJ Faulin-Fajardo.
Bioestadística amigable, 2ª ed., Ediciones Díaz de Santos, (2006),
[50]
J Decety.
The neuroevolution of empathy.
Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1231 (2011), pp. 35-45

Conflictos de interés: Los autores manifiestan que no tienen conflictos de interés en este artículo.

Financiado por Colciencias, mediante Contrato No. 455-2009. Datos derivados del proyecto Procesamiento emocional y su modulación en personas reinsertadas del conflicto colombiano, pertenecientes a la alta consejería de la Presidencia de la República, zona Antioquia.

Copyright © 2013. Asociación Colombiana de Psiquiatría
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo