The aim of this study was to measure the eight Psychological Types proposed by Carl G. Jung in Mexican population. A cross-sectional, multicenter, exploratory study was carried out, for which a psychometric instrument based on Jungian typology theory was developed. The Psychological Type Indicator (ITP, for its acronym in Spanish) consisted of 8 subscales (one per psychological type). It was administered to a sample of 1194 participants and validated by the methodological and statistical procedure developed by Reyes Lagunes and García y Barragán. The participants were Mexican university students, ranging from 17 to 40 years (median=21, IQR=4; 63% female). The internal consistency of the instrument was determined, obtaining an alpha coefficient of 0.873. An exploratory factor analysis indicated a structure of 10 factors, which together explained 32.672% of the variance. Four of the found factors were consistent with Jungian typology (Introverted Feeling, Introverted Thinking, Extraverted Sensing and Extraverted Feeling). The rest of the factors grouped attributes of this theory, which enable a new way of assessing personality in Mexican population with reference on this model of personality.
El objetivo de este estudio fue medir los 8 tipos psicológicos propuestos por Carl G. Jung en población mexicana. Se llevó a cabo un estudio transversal, multicéntrico y exploratorio, en el cual se desarrolló un instrumento basado en la tipología junguiana. El Indicador de Tipos Psicológicos (ITP) se conformó por 8 subescalas (una por tipo psicológico). El instrumento fue administrado a una muestra de 1,194 estudiantes universitarios mexicanos provenientes de 3 estados distintos de la República Mexicana (Guanajuato, Jalisco y la Ciudad de México), y se validó por medio de la propuesta metodológico-estadística de Reyes Lagunes y García y Barragán. Los participantes fueron hombres y mujeres (37 y 63%, respectivamente) de entre 17 y 40 años de edad (mediana=21, RIC=4). Se determinó la consistencia interna del instrumento, obteniéndose un coeficiente alfa de 0.873. El análisis factorial exploratorio arrojó una estructura factorial compuesta por 10 factores, que en conjunto explicó el 32.672% de la varianza. De los 10 factores encontrados, 4 fueron consistentes con la tipología junguiana (sentimiento introvertido, pensamiento introvertido, sensación extravertida y sentimiento extravertido). El resto de los factores agruparon atributos de la teoría junguiana, los cuales permiten una nueva forma de evaluar la personalidad en México de forma válida y confiable, con referencia a este modelo de personalidad.
The Psychological Types model proposed by Carl G. Jung in the 1920s posits the existence of eight characteristic types of a persistent general attitude, understanding as “attitude” the disposition of the mind for acting in a certain form and direction, in a predetermined way (Jung, 1995 [1921]). The theory assumes the existence of innate preferences for gaining energy, gathering information, taking decisions and, in general, orienting oneself toward the world (Tucker, 2010). Additionally, this theoretical approach tries to understand the point of view from which the individual experiences the unconscious (Beebe, 2004). Thus, Jung suggests two fundamental attitudes: introversion and extraversion, which are originated by the concepts of “object” and “subject”. Jung defines the “object” and the “objective” as everything that belongs to the real, external and tangible world; on the other hand, the “subject” and the “subjective” represents conceptions that corresponds fundamentally to the person's psyche (Jung, 1995 [1921]). According to the theory, four functions emerge: Thinking, Feeling, Sensing and Intuition. The functions involve the way the individual relates to the immediate surrounding, and how information is extracted from the environment (Robertson, 2006). If each attitude is combined with a different function, eight psychological types will be obtained (Fig. 1).
Since its creation, this theory has been embraced by a large number of professionals of different areas of psychology. It has proven being fruitful in the therapeutic context, and clinically useful for providing a framework to understand individual differences in behavior (Osmond, Siegler, & Smoke, 1977). However, an increasing number of psychology professionals have questioned this theory in the past decades, as its empirical support has not been fully demonstrated (Loomis, 1982). This Jungian personality model has been difficult to operationalize and to empirically demonstrate (Davis & Mattoon, 2006). According to some authors, Jung himself found difficult “typifying” his patients, coming to believe that his typology could have been just a wordplay (Osmond et al., 1977). Other difficulties revolves around accurately understanding the introverted attitude from an objective perspective, since it depends strongly of archetypes and mental images that cannot be easily communicated (McCrae & Costa-Jr, 1989). Likewise, the descriptions given by Jung about each type make strong reference to the unconsciousness, propitiating that attitudes and functions overlap with each other (McCrae & Costa-Jr, 1989). A study conducted by Cook (1970) indicated the only genuine bipolarity in this typology was extraversion/introversion. Moreover, Gray (1947) also pointed a temporal instability in the theory, finding that, in a lapse of 5 years, people tends to use more sensation and thinking than in their youth, and also develop a more introverted attitude.
In spite of these difficulties, psychometric instruments that measure Jungian typology have been developed. The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) is the most widely used test based in this theory (Matoon & Davis, 1995). It measures 16 Psychological Types instead of eight, since two additional dimensions that were added by Katherine Briggs and Isabel Briggs-Myers to Jung's original model: “Judging” and “Perceiving”. Judging focuses in a preferred lifestyle in the sense of order, meanwhile Perceiving indicates a preference for a less structured approach to life (Arnau, Green, Rosen, Gleaves, & Melancon, 2003). This 16-type structure was later used in several subsequent Jungian typology instruments. Despite its extensive use and its influence on other test, the MBTI has been target of numerous controversies. The authors of the test, Katherine Briggs and Isabel Briggs-Myers were outsiders from the scientific community, thus, their incursion into the field of personality assessment has been questioned by theorists and researchers (Cranton & Knoop, 1995). Furthermore, the instrument is not isomorphic with the theory on which it is based, since it measures 16 types and not eight. Therefore, using the MBTI to measure Jungian typology must be considered with caution (McCrae & Costa-Jr, 1989).
Referring to the psychometric aspects of the MBTI, some studies have evidenced the low test-retest reliability, showing that, even when the interval between applications is short (5 weeks), about 50% of respondents will fall into a different MBTI type (Pittenger, 1993). In addition, it is called into question the relevance of reducing each type in a dichotomous scales, since Jung acknowledges the existence of intermediate positions between attitudes and functions, whereas in the MBTI these are mutually exclusive (McCrae & Costa-Jr, 1989). Studies conducted have questioned the bipolarity of the theory, claiming that the functions should be measured independently instead of a forced-response format (Loomis, 1982). Myers and McCaulley, (cited by Vacha-Haase & Thompson, 2002) suggest that the MBTI scores were designed to show the direction of the preference, not its intensity. Other authors consider the forced-response format as a “statistical artifact that will inherently yield spurious negative correlations between the answers to the items” (Girelli & Stake, 1993).
A way to obtain evidence for the validity of a psychometric instrument and the theory on which it is based is by a factor analysis that reflects its underlying constructs (Morales Vallejo, 2013). Factor analysis of previous inventories designed to measure Jung's typology, failed in evidencing that the tests measured what it intended to measure (Loomis, 1982). Without this evidence, no psychometric instrument that purports measuring this theory will have the necessary empirical basis required for experimental research or clinical application. In Mexico, no studies that intend to measure Jungian typology have been developed. Even though there is a Spanish translated version of the MBTI, it is not adapted nor validated specifically for Mexican population, hence, it lacks of semantic and cultural relevance (García y Barragán, García Campos, & Reyes Lagunes, 2017). The Ethical Code of the Psychologist states explicitly the psychologist's obligation of using culturally relevant assessment techniques in its 18th article (Sociedad Mexicana de Psicología, 2010).
Given the above, the following research question was formulated: Does the eight original Psychological Types proposed by Carl G. Jung exist in Mexican population? Measuring the typology in this specific context will allow to obtain evidence of its validity for the first time in this country. The findings by this study could serve as a guideline for the mental health professional when choosing an assessment method. It is necessary to have methods of psychological assessment that are supported by solid scientific evidence, and, in addition, that are semantically and culturally relevant to the particular geographical context in which they will be applied (García y Barragán et al., 2017). In addition, the test resulting from this study can serve as an alternative to the MBTI, since its use without certification or its modification might result in a conflict with the MBTI publishers.
MethodParticipantsThe sample consisted of 1194 Mexican University students from three different schools, each one located in different cities of the country. Four hundred and forty five men (37.3%) and 749 women (62.7%) participated. The age range was between 17 and 40 years old (median=20, IQR=4). According to the declared sexual orientation, 1086 participants indicated being heterosexual (men=87.4%, women=93%), 39 homosexual (men=5.6%, women=1.8%) and 41 bisexual (men=3.3%, women=3.4%); 28 participants did not indicate their sexual orientation (men=3.5%, women=1.6%). The students belonged to 40 different academic programs, both undergraduate and graduate. About half of the sample (52.1%) corresponded to academic programs from the health sciences area, from which the 25% were psychology students.
InstrumentThe Psychological Type Indicator (ITP) consisted of eight sub-scales (one per psychological type), each one composed by 12 items, giving a total of 96 items (Table 1). Its reliability was calculated by a previous pilot study, where an alpha coefficient of 0.909 was obtained. Each item corresponded to a statement that reflected a specific characteristic of a type. The participant indicated the level of agreement or disagreement with the statement through a Likert scale, where 1 represented total disagreement and 5 indicated a total agreement.
ITP subscales.
Subscale | Items |
---|---|
Extraverted Thinking | 11 – 12 – 15 – 28 – 38 – 55 – 58 – 75 – 83 – 87 – 89 – 91 |
Introverted Thinking | 1 – 6 – 13 – 16 – 24 – 37 – 40 – 65 – 69 – 79 – 82 – 90 |
Extraverted Feeling | 14 – 30 – 45 – 51 – 57 – 59 – 61 – 80 – 84 – 86 – 92 – 93 |
Introverted Feeling | 2 – 4 – 19 – 22 – 33 – 43 – 47 – 50 – 52 – 66 – 70 – 72 |
Extraverted Sensing | 25 – 27 – 44 – 46 – 54 – 56 – 60 – 68 – 77 – 81 – 85 – 95 |
Introverted Sensing | 3 – 5 – 10 – 21 – 31 – 49 – 63 – 71 – 73 – 88 – 94 – 96 |
Extraverted Intuition | 8 – 17 – 23 – 26 – 32 – 35 – 36 – 39 – 41 – 53 – 62 – 67 |
Introverted Intuition | 7 – 9 – 18 – 20 – 29 – 34 – 42 – 48 – 64 – 74 – 76 – 78 |
Note: Subscales that originally conformed the first version of the ITP along with the item numbers that conformed each subscale.
This was a cross-sectional, multicentric and exploratory study, since this was the first approach from the typology toward Mexican Population. The first step was the development of a set of 172 items that were later evaluated by 5 judges. Only the items in which all judges manifested agreement remained in the test, remaining a total of 114 items. The pilot test was administered to a sample of 163 university students, and with these results, a preliminary validation of the test was implemented to make the necessary corrections, and then obtaining the final version of the instrument. The test was administered in person in three different Mexican universities. In order to guarantee the confidentiality of the data, any personal information was omitted, and instead, a different identification number was added to each test. Special emphasis was made on the voluntary nature of the student's participation, and in the absence of negative consequences if the student refused to participate. The research protocol was revised and approved by the Research Committee of the University of Guanajuato.
ResultsFor the validation of the instrument, the methodological proposal by Reyes Lagunes and García y Barragán (2008) was implemented. A frequency analysis was made to verify that every response option in each item was scored; on the contrary, it should be eliminated. In the case of the ITP, it was not necessary to eliminate any item in this step, since all of the choice options resulted attractive for the participants. A t-test for independent samples was then carried out to ensure that every item was able to discriminate between low and high scores. According to this step, 9 items were eliminated for not evidencing such discrimination. Reliability was analyzed through Cronbach's Alpha, obtaining a coefficient of 0.873 for the whole scale, which reflects good internal consistency. A Pearson's correlation was implemented to determine the type of rotation to use in the incoming exploratory factor analysis. Correlations were low, thus, an exploratory factor analysis with orthogonal rotation was carried out. A structure composed by 10 factors (Table 2) was obtained, which collectively explained the 32.672% of variance. Four of those 10 factors were congruent with Jung's Typology: Introverted Feeling, Introverted Thinking, Extraverted Feeling and Extraverted Sensing (Table 3).
Final factor structure.
Item | F1 | F2 | F3 | F4 | F5 | F6 | F7 | F8 | F9 | F10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
43 I find it hard explaining my feelings to others | .780 | .096 | .060 | .155 | .097 | .136 | −.016 | −.010 | .056 | .043 |
33 I can’t find a way for expressing my feelings | .747 | .071 | .077 | .114 | .114 | .095 | −.065 | .006 | .128 | .027 |
19 Is hard for me explaining my emotions with words | .741 | .013 | −.024 | .067 | .141 | −.009 | −.004 | .016 | .048 | −.014 |
47 Is hard for opening myself to others | .659 | .253 | −.009 | −.051 | .074 | .023 | .012 | .033 | −.065 | .011 |
52 I rather reserving my feelings | .652 | .257 | .034 | .018 | .044 | .053 | .033 | .034 | .008 | −.056 |
69 I don’t say what I think | .544 | .157 | .273 | −.001 | .074 | .075 | −.034 | .153 | .034 | −.065 |
86 Sometimes I don’t say what I really think | .476 | .076 | .316 | .050 | −.042 | .187 | .119 | −.072 | −.125 | −.023 |
79 I think I’m a cold person | .444 | .211 | −.184 | .035 | .079 | .121 | −.173 | −.019 | .174 | −.031 |
24 I like more being alone tan surrounded by people | .207 | .783 | −.071 | .032 | .047 | .040 | −.067 | .014 | .119 | −.052 |
21 Being isolated satisfies me | .253 | .683 | .060 | −.003 | .062 | .096 | .002 | −.035 | .007 | −.049 |
90 Being surrounded by others exhausts me | .112 | .612 | −.041 | .111 | .134 | .125 | −.027 | −.044 | .044 | −.017 |
1 I rather working alone than in team | .064 | .525 | .131 | .085 | .026 | .033 | .029 | −.100 | .077 | .041 |
93 People's criticisms affect me | .066 | .057 | .690 | .090 | .159 | .200 | −.002 | −.085 | −.022 | −.075 |
84 I’m willing to say what others wants to hear in order to being accepted | .195 | .018 | .586 | −.039 | .092 | .047 | −.278 | .001 | .127 | −.017 |
57 Feeling appreciated by others is important for me | −.107 | −.026 | .582 | .084 | −.048 | .040 | .124 | .208 | −.066 | .027 |
92 I take care of others more than myself | .068 | .007 | .534 | .119 | .161 | .082 | −.014 | −.002 | −.077 | .059 |
9 I’m very impulsive | .087 | .057 | .028 | .785 | .036 | .008 | −.010 | .094 | −.008 | .001 |
42 I take decisions impulsively | .153 | .021 | .126 | .755 | .155 | .071 | −.060 | .050 | .024 | .098 |
4 I explode when I have intense feelings | .024 | .110 | .098 | .525 | .010 | .135 | .109 | .052 | −.001 | .112 |
3 I don’t enjoy my routine | −.096 | .034 | −.027 | −.010 | −.684 | −.018 | −.040 | −.038 | .007 | −.018 |
18 I can’t adapt to my daily grind | .158 | .128 | .130 | .125 | .650 | .114 | −.110 | −.045 | .150 | −.018 |
40 I feel like an outsider in this world | .210 | .360 | .039 | .016 | .464 | .181 | −.002 | −.046 | −.098 | .061 |
31 I’m an unstable person | −.130 | −.129 | −.111 | −.245 | −.427 | −.075 | .111 | .100 | .140 | .004 |
16 I find it hard adapting to my surroundings | .311 | .235 | .100 | .083 | .409 | .048 | .010 | −.041 | −.001 | −.175 |
94 I usually get lost into details | .105 | .126 | .210 | .026 | .073 | .667 | .009 | .080 | −.084 | .044 |
95 Sometimes, insignificant details distract me from what I do | .122 | .063 | .134 | .087 | .105 | .646 | .093 | .089 | −.103 | .065 |
81 Is hard for me to listen other's advices | .202 | .025 | .047 | .181 | .120 | .472 | −.052 | −.123 | .215 | −.111 |
34 People say I have my head in the clouds | .167 | .099 | .094 | .301 | .191 | .419 | −.084 | −.002 | −.059 | .109 |
54 I like the feeling of being outside | −.030 | .001 | −.136 | −.019 | −.001 | .091 | .661 | .115 | −.081 | .116 |
55 I take decisions according to my current circumstances | .046 | −.045 | .067 | .076 | .071 | .017 | .568 | .169 | .004 | .026 |
46 Actions are more important than words | −.013 | −.026 | .110 | .032 | −.116 | −.184 | .434 | .315 | .006 | .204 |
38 Is better knowing a Little of everything than a lot of only one thing | .052 | −.068 | .072 | .049 | −.007 | .007 | .125 | .650 | .072 | −.096 |
60 I need experimenting things in order to understand them | .066 | .033 | .063 | .114 | .018 | .204 | .021 | .636 | .070 | .253 |
96 I think experience is the best way for gaining knowledge | .016 | −.013 | −.045 | .057 | −.055 | .013 | .198 | .620 | −.044 | .124 |
25 I give importance only to what is concrete and real | −.007 | .110 | −.071 | −.073 | −.047 | −.044 | .041 | .156 | .708 | .017 |
44 I consider important only what is tangible | .151 | .052 | .079 | .090 | .042 | −.040 | −.071 | −.004 | .699 | .081 |
64 I think everything can be rationally understand | .066 | .001 | .156 | .089 | −.126 | .070 | .212 | .189 | −.415 | .064 |
62 I feel more attracted to new than conventional ideas | .009 | −.097 | .045 | .023 | .015 | .027 | .059 | .231 | .047 | .676 |
67 I like taking risks | −.081 | −.068 | −.116 | .125 | .027 | .016 | .039 | .051 | −.021 | .561 |
48 I dislike jobs that limit my creativity | .024 | .088 | .010 | .017 | −.086 | .078 | .282 | −.064 | .044 | .552 |
Cronbach's Alpha | .831 | .707 | .608 | .667 | .171 | .622 | .540 | .540 | .064 | .433 |
Mean | 22.20 | 10.70 | 10.85 | 8.75 | 13.89 | 11.03 | 12.42 | 11.40 | 9.24 | 10.24 |
Standard deviation | 6.58 | 3.35 | 3.061 | 2.916 | 2.305 | 3.154 | 1.992 | 2.309 | 1.906 | 2.176 |
Note: F=factor. Factor loadings>.40 are on bold. Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. The rotation converged in 41 iterations.
Factors congruent with Jung's Typology.
Factor | Items that belong to the factor | Subscale for which the item was originally created |
---|---|---|
1 | 43 I find it hard explaining my feelings to others | Introverted Feeling |
33 I can’t find a way for expressing my feelings | ||
19 Is hard for me explaining my emotions with words | ||
47 Is hard for opening myself to others | ||
52 I rather reserving my feelings | ||
69 I don’t say what I think | ||
86 Sometimes I don’t say what I really think | ||
79 I think I’m a cold person | ||
2 | 24 I like more being alone tan surrounded by people | Introverted Thinking |
21 Being isolated satisfies me | ||
90 Being surrounded by others exhausts me | ||
1 I rather working alone than in team | ||
3 | 93 People's criticisms affect me | Extraverted Feeling |
84 I’m willing to say what others wants to hear in order to being accepted | ||
57 Feeling appreciated by others is important for me | ||
92 I take care of others more than myself | ||
9 | 25 I give importance only to what is concrete and real | Extraverted Sensing |
44 I consider important only what is tangible | ||
64 I think everything can be rationally understand |
Note: Factors that were congruent with the original Jung's typology after the exploratory factor analysis.
The remaining factors found in this study grouped, mostly, items belonging to the same attitude (introverted/extraverted) without recognizing between functions; which coincides with Cook's findings (1970), since he concluded that only this bipolarity seems to be genuine. Cronbach's alpha for the resulting factor structure, which finally was composed of 40 items, resulted in 0.808 coefficient, indicating again good internal consistency. The factors lacking of theoretical congruence were re-evaluated qualitatively, this in order to determine the common measurement trait among items, and it was subsequently recoded (Table 4). This represents a reinterpretation of the theory, resulting in a proposal to measure personality through attributes of Jungian typology.
Factor recoding.
Factor | Item | Subscale for which the item was created | Recoding |
---|---|---|---|
4 | 9 I’m very impulsive | Introverted Intuition | Introverted Impulsivity |
42 I take decisions impulsively | Introverted Intuition | ||
4 I explode when I have intense feelings | Extraverted Feeling | ||
5 | 3 I don’t enjoy my routine | Extraverted Intuition | Introverted Dereliction |
18 I can’t adapt to my daily grind | Introverted Intuition | ||
40 I feel like an outsider in this world | Introverted Thinking | ||
31 I’m an unstable person | Extraverted Intuition | ||
16 I find it hard adapting to my surroundings | Introverted Thinking | ||
6 | 94 I usually get lost into details | Introverted Sensing | Introverted Precision |
95 Sometimes, insignificant details distract me from what I do | Extraverted Sensing | ||
81 Is hard for me to listen other's advices | Extraverted Sensing | ||
34 People say I have my head in the clouds | Introverted Intuition | ||
7 | 54 I like the feeling of being outside | Extraverted Sensing | Extraverted Realism |
55 I take decisions according to my current circumstances | Extraverted Thinking | ||
46 Actions are more important than words | Extraverted Sensing | ||
8 | 38 Is better knowing a Little of everything than a lot of only one thing | Extraverted Thinking | Extraverted Practicity |
60 I need experimenting things in order to understand them | Extraverted Sensing | ||
96 I think experience is the best way for gaining knowledge | Introverted Sensing | ||
10 | 62 I feel more attracted to new than conventional ideas | Extraverted Intuition | Extraverted Audacity |
67 I like taking risks | Extraverted Intuition | ||
48 I dislike jobs that limit my creativity | Introverted Intuition |
Note: Factors that grouped items from different scales from which they were created for. These factors were individually assessed to determine the similarities within the items that they gathered for their posterior recoding.
Percentiles of the 10 factors were obtained (Table 5), this in order to determine the intensity of the preference of each type: Scores below the 20th percentile indicated a very low preference, below the 40th percentile indicated low preference, below the 60th indicated a medium preference, below the 80th percentile indicated a high preference, and from percentile 81 and above will be considered as a very high preference.
Jung posited that one of the types, the Introverted Sensing, is more common in women (Jung, 1995 [1921]), and later Robertson (2006) retook to this issue claiming that it is also highly prevalent in homosexual men. However, in the present study, it was found that only 32% of homosexual men show high or very high scores in this psychological type. In the case of women, 35.7% presented high or very high scores. In both cases, scores were distributed more or less homogeneously in all levels of preference (Table 6).
Introverted Sensing Type scores for women and homosexual men.
Women (mean, %) | Homosexual men (mean, %) | ||
---|---|---|---|
Very high | 117 (17.25%) | Very high | 4 (16%) |
High | 126 (18.5%) | High | 4 (16%) |
Medium | 152 (22.4%) | Medium | 5 (20%) |
Low | 116 (17.1%) | Low | 6 (16%) |
Very low | 167 (24.6%) | Very low | 6 (16%) |
Total | 678 (100%) | Total | 25 (100%) |
Note: Comparison of the proportion of Introverted Sensing scores between women and homosexual men, on its different levels of preference.
Jungian typology has stimulated a considerable quantity of research, nevertheless, research focused on verifying the typology per se still insufficient. Studies that have evaluated the validity of various instruments in Jungian typology have shown similar results to this research, since the found factor structures have been only partially consistent with the theory (Arnau, Rosen, & Thompson, 2000; Davis & Mattoon, 2006; Johnson, Mauzey, Johnson, & Murphy, 2001; Loomis, 1982; Macdonald & Holland, 1993; Sipps, Alexander, & Friedt, 1985; Woehlke & Piper, 1980). The causes for these results can be viewed from several perspectives. There is a possibility of some flaw with the instruments that are designed to measure the theory, resulting in a failure to properly assess the types (Loomis, 1982). Moreover, it is possible that Jung's theory contains structural problems that are blocking the progress in its understanding and validation (Metzner, Burney, & Mahlberg, 1981). The results of previous research, together with the present findings, seem to indicate the last point.
It is necessary to question if researchers have been actually failing periodically, even with different instruments and different methodologies, in properly measuring the typology. The evidence seems to indicate that the theory has failed in establishing clear differences between types, and this could be due their similarities, since they share common characteristics proper of the introverted or extraverted attitude, and in the way in which the types manifest the unconscious. Therefore, as evidenced by the present investigation, it is possible to assess the personality by certain attributes of Jungian Typology, but the original structure proposed by Jung seems to be flawed. The question that could arise now is, how is that instruments based in Jung's Typology continue being so extensively used? A possible answer could be the high facie validity of the instrument, since, both users and appliers, can easily identify themselves or others with one of the types (Vacha-Haase & Thompson, 2002). Besides, this theory gives the same value to every single type: each one of them is worthy and none is better than other. The MBTI is one of the most popular personality assessment tools worldwide (Shi & Yang, 2009). Its popularity could be erroneously interpreted as an indicator of its accuracy and utility, which propitiates its indiscriminate use without even questioning the test's basis (Pittenger, 1993). It is also important to consider the economic interests involved, since, to have access to the MBTI, it is necessary to go through a certification process that implies a considerable financial investment (Cunningham, 2012).
On the subject of the Introverted Feeling type and its high prevalence in women and homosexual men, it is worth mentioning that the assumptions made by Jung and Robertson may correspond only to stigmas proper of the time in which the theory was developed, and that will not necessarily reflect nowadays’ reality. Nevertheless, it is suggested to carry out subsequent studies that tests these particular hypotheses. Like any research, ours is not exempt of weaknesses. The sampling was uneven in the proportion of men and women, in the age of the participants and in their academic program. In subsequent studies, it is suggested a more randomized sampling method, and also to include open or clinical population, since several other studies investigating this topic have only included university students. Note that the students participating in this project were subjected to a screening process for entering to their respective programs of study, as part of their admissions process, which could be influencing the results obtained in the present study. Even with these weaknesses, and since this was an exploratory research, we strongly believe that this first approach of the theory to Mexican population can be useful for further studies with different designs.
For a copy of the final test that resulted from this validation and its manual, please contact the correspondent author.
FundingFunding was obtained through the “Call of support for transversal research projects, 2013”, from the University of Guanajuato.
Conflict of interestThe authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
To each and every single one of the students and professors, who kindly gave us their time and willingness for participating on this study.
Peer Review under the responsibility of Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México.