covid
Buscar en
Journal of Behavior, Health & Social Issues
Toda la web
Inicio Journal of Behavior, Health & Social Issues Effect of the induction of moderate hyperprolactiniemia on sexual behavior in wi...
Información de la revista
Vol. 7. Núm. 1.
Páginas 9-17 (mayo - octubre 2015)
Compartir
Compartir
Descargar PDF
Más opciones de artículo
Visitas
3637
Vol. 7. Núm. 1.
Páginas 9-17 (mayo - octubre 2015)
Open Access
Effect of the induction of moderate hyperprolactiniemia on sexual behavior in wistar male rats
Efecto de la inducción de una hiperprolactinemia moderada sobre la conducta sexual de ratas macho de la cepa wistar
Visitas
3637
Luz Irene Pascual-Mathey1,2,
Autor para correspondencia
lupascual@uv.mx

Correspondence.
, Sara Rodríguez Romero1, Pablo Becerra Lara2, Jorge Manzo1, Maria Elena Hernández1
1 Center of Brain Research, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Ver., México
2 Faculty of Biological Pharmaceutical Chemistry, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Ver., México
Este artículo ha recibido

Under a Creative Commons license
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Figuras (4)
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Tablas (1)
Table 1. Sexual behavior parameters.
Abstract

At physiological high levels, Prolactin (PRL) is related with many functions, including lactation, reproduction and sexual behavior. Abnormal high levels, referred to as hyperprolactinemia (HP), are associated with sexual disorders like hypogonadism, erectile dysfunction and infertility. Male rats with HP display less mounts and intromissions, as well as higher ejaculation latencies, however, little is known about the effect of modest HP. To learn more about this topic, the study aims to analyze the effect of a moderate HP (40 ng/ml), induced by pituitary transplantation and ovine PRL administration (100μg/d), on sexual behavior of sexually experienced male rats. Sixty-four sexually experienced male rats were randomly assigned to 4 groups: 1) Ctrl; 2) oPRL; 3) Graft and; 4) Sham. During a time of 15 days, rats were subjected to a sexual behavior test every three days. The last day, sexual behavior was tested with the SBR Software. Results showed that pituitary graft produced and increase in the number of mounts, and longer intromission-ejaculation latencies compared with controls. Also, we found that hit rate was reduced. Interestingly, these parameters are related with the anticipatory/consummatory phases of the sexual behavior, suggesting a role of PRL in males with not chronic HP.

Keywords:
Prolactin
sexual behavior
hyperprolactinemia
sexual dysfunction
Resumen

A niveles fisiológicos altos, Prolactina (PRL) está envuelta en muchas funciones, incluidas la lactancia, reproducción y la conducta sexual. Por otro lado, los niveles anormales altos, condición conocida como Hiperprolactinemia (HP), están asociados con desórdenes sexuales como hipogonadismo, disfunción eréctil e infertilidad. Ratas macho con HP presentan menos montas e intromisiones, así como latencias de eyaculación más largas, sin embargo, poco se sabe acerca del effecto de los incrementos moderados de PRL sobre la conducta sexual. Con el in de aprender más sobre este tema, este estudio busca analizar el effecto de la HP moderada (40 ng/ml), inducida por el trasplante hipoisario y la administración de PRL ovina (100μg/d), sobre la conducta sexual en ratas macho sexualmente expertas. Para ello, sesenta y cuatro ratas macho con experiencia sexual fueron aleatoriamente asignadas a 4 grupos: 1) Ctrl; 2) oPRL; 3) Graft y; 4) Sham. Durante un periodo de 15 días, las ratas fueron sometidas a una evaluación de conducta sexual cada tres días. El último día, se registró la conducta sexual con ayuda del software SBR. Los resultados mostraron que el trasplante hipoisario produjo un incremento en el número de montas, con latencias de intromisión mucho más largas comparadas con los controles, así como una disminución significativa en el hit rate. De forma interesante, estos parámetros están relacionados con las fases anticipatorias/consumatorias de la conducta sexual, sugiriendo un rol de PRL en machos con HP no crónica. Palabras clave: Prolactina, conducta sexual, hiperprolactinemia, disfunción sexual..

Palabras clave:
Prolactina
conducta sexual
hiperprolactinemia
disfunción sexual
Texto completo
Introduction

Physiologic Hyperprolactinemia (HP) is associated with reproductive processes, like breastfeeding and pregnancy. However, at pathological conditions, it participates in the development of galactorrhea, amenorrhea, and erectile dysfunction (ED). This latter is understood as the inability to keep an erection during the sexual intercourse, which is associated with a decrease of both sexual arousal and libido (Bernichtein, Touraine, & Goin, 2010; Ben-Jonathan, LaPensee, & LaPensee, 2008; Molitch, & Reichlin, 1982).

Sexual behavior is regulated by hormonal and dopaminergic mechanisms (Kruger, Hartmann, & Schedlowski, 2005). In this sense, some hormones, e.g., Progesterone (P), Testosterone (T), Estradiol (E), Luteinizing Hormone (LH) and Prolactin (PRL), work together in order to keep sexual behavior regulation. Accordingly, the endocrine system plays an essential role in the control and adequate execution of this activity; for that reason, a chronic decrease in T levels observed in patients with hypogonadism is associated with ED (Veronesi et al., 2011; Martinez-Jabaloyas, Queipo-Zaragoza, Pastor-Hernandez, Gil-Salom, & Chuan-Nuez, 2006). Moreover, the chronic HP (>100ng/ml) has been observed in most cases of ED, suggesting a negative effect of PRL on sexual behavior, in rats and humans (Abram et al., 1998; Doherty, Bartke, & Smith, 1985; Katovich, Cameron, Murray, & Gunsalus, 1985).

Previous studies from our group showed that hormonal levels of PRL and T increased significantly in sexually experienced male rats subjected to consecutive ejaculations (Hernandez et al., 2006; Hernandez et al., 2007). Likewise, in humans, PRL levels increased after orgasm, remaining high for a while, suggesting that temporal increases in PRL play an important role in facilitative effects and in the control of sexual arousal after orgasm (Hernandez et al., 2007; Kruger, Haake, Hartmann, Schedlowski, & Exton, 2002).

However, the chronic HP is associated with a significant reduction in the gonadal function in men and women, suggesting that PRL is a peripheral regulatory factor essential in reproductive functions (Kruger et al., 2002). What is more, PRL function is conditioned to dopamine (DA) in a dose-dependent manner (Manna, El Hefnawy, Kero, & Huhtaniemi, 2001). When the DA antagonist domperidone was injected (4 mg/kg) to male rats, the HP induced (>60-70 ng/ml), correlated with facilitative effects within 5 days, related to shorter ejaculation latencies when compared with controls

Conversely, when domperidone was injected for 30 days, opposite effects were observed, related to longer ejaculation and intromissions periods (Nasello, Vanzeler, Madureira, & Felicio, 1997). In this context, the induction of a chronic HP (>250 ng/ml) for 18 days, is enough to produce a negative effect on sexual behavior (Doherty, Bartke, Smith, & Davis, 1985). Moreover, when PRL is injected directly into the brain of the rats, facilitative effects were observed in only five hours, while the inhibitory effects were observed in five days, suggesting a direct effect of PRL when is released on the Central Nervous System (CNS) (Cruz-Casallas, Nasello, Hucke, & Felicio, 1999; Nasello et al., 1997).

In accordance with these results, previous studies developed by Nasello et al., showed that PRL is able to induce changes in the dopaminergic activity. Thus, the regulation of sexual behavior may be occurring through a negative feedback of PRL to dopaminergic neurons in the CNS (Kruger et al., 2005; Nasello et al., 1997).

The most of patients with sexual dysfunction, showed a strong hypogonadism (Martinez-Jabaloyas, et al., 2006), however, the studies described above suggested that PRL and its inhibitor (DA), are essentials to produce a dysregulation of sexual behavior, in which, PRL could be decreasing DA levels in specific brain areas, like the nucleus accumbens, paraventricular nucleus, medial preoptic area (MPOA), and the striatum (Martinez-Jabaloyas et al., 2006; Giuliano & Allard, 2001a).

All these data suggest that the effect of PRL on sexual behavior is mediated by high and sustained PRL levels. Conversely, in order to observe changes in a short time, is necessary the administration of PRL directly in the CNS to modify the sexual behavior parameters (Cruz-Casallas et al., 1999).

However, data obtained in a parallel study in our lab, showed that moderate increases in PRL levels were enough to induce changes in the morphology of the prostatic tissue and, hence, could be able to modify the sexual behavior in sexually experienced male rats (Hernandez et al., 2006). For that reason, the present study aims to analyze the acute increments in prolactin levels on sexual behavior of male rats subjected to fifteen treatment days.

Materials and MethodsAnimals and housing

Three-month old Wistar male rats were used for this study. Animals were subjected to an inverted lightdark cycle (12-12 h, lights of at 8:00 AM) within plastic cages (50 × 30 × 20 cm) containing wood chip bedding. Water and food (Harlan Mexico, rodent chow) were available ad libitum. Animal care during experimentation was conducted according to the Mexican official standard NOM-062-ZOO-1999, and under the Society for Neuroscience policy on the uses of animals in neuroscience research.

Sexual training

Sixty-four sexually experienced male rats were used in this experiment (Garcia-Martinez et al., 2010). Previously, these animals were subjected to a sexual behavior training every tree days. Males were placed in a plexiglas arena (60 centimeters of diameter × 60 centimeters of high), five minutes for habituation. Immediately, a receptive female (OVX with previous injection of benzoate de estradiol [10 μg; SIGMA] and progesterone [2 mg; SIGMA], 48 and 4 hours before the test), was introduced in the arena until the male achieved the ejaculation (20 minutes maximum). After four sessions, subjects who got an ejaculation between five to ten minutes were randomly assigned to four groups of sixteen rats each one: 1) Ctrl (saline solution injection); 2) oPRL (ovine PRL injection, SIGMA, Cat. L6520, 50 μg each 12 hours); 3) Graft (pituitary transplantation) and; 4) Sham (false surgery).

HP induction

Moderate Hyperprolactinemia (HP) was induced by oPRL injection (IP, 100 μg dairy) for the oPRL group, and pituitary transplantation to the kidney for the graft group (Sato et al., 1997). Transplantation was carried out obtaining the pituitary from female donors by quickly decapitation and keeping them in sterile culture dishes containing saline solution. Simultaneously, males were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (Smith Kline, Mexico; 30 mg/kg b.w, IP), and the left kidney was exposed through a dorsal skin incision. Pituitary was introduced carefully under the renal capsule. Animals were kept during one week in recuperation before the beginning of sexual behavior test. In the sham group, the same procedure was performed, without introducing the pituitary (false surgery). The main diference between treatments is the plasmatic PRL level, since with oPRL administration we observed significant pulses (˜40ng/ml) in PRL levels after administration in the morning and night, returning to the physiological state (˜12ng/ml) after a while, whereas with the transplant, HP was maintained during the entire treatment (Abram et al., 1998).

Sexual behavior assessment

The stereotyped pattern of male copulatory activity in rats led to development of parameters that are useful in the analysis of male sexual behavior. This parameters involve the implementation of behavioral responses without penile insertion, called mounts (M); characteristic movements with intravaginal penile insertion, called intromissions (I) and, characteristic movements of penile insertion with ejaculation (E) (Manzo et al., 2002 [a;b]).

Sexual behavior was recorded with the Sexual Behavior Recorder Software (SBR), during the dark period (12:00 and 16:00 h) (Larsson, 1956). Males were placed in the arena with a receptive female, and the index of mount and intromissions was measured during the sexual intercourse (Paredes, & Agmo, 2004). The experiment was performed every three days (sessions 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5). The parameters recorded were: I) mount frequency (total number of mounts made by a male, measured by the number of mounts during the sexual intercourse); II) intromission frequency (total number of intromissions made by the male, measured by the number of intromissions during the sexual intercourse); III) first mount latency (is the time from the onset of sexual behavior until the first mount occurs, measured in seconds); IV) first intromission-ejaculation latency (time since the first intromission until the male ejaculates; measured in seconds), and; V) hit rate (sum of the number of mounts and intromissions, divided by the sum of the number of mounts, intromissions and ejaculations [I+E/I+M+E]). Hit rate is a measure of sexual behavior performance, with values above 0.5 indicating a good copulatory activity (Manzo et al., 2002 [a;b]).

PRL quantification

Blood samples were obtained at the end of last session by heart puncture. Previously, males were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (30 mg/kg BW, IP), and blood was obtained using sterile syringes. Ten samples were collected from each group, and they were centrifuged, separated into aliquots and stored at -20°C. PRL levels were determined by the immunoassay PRL ELISA kit ALPCO (Cat. 12-MKVRP1) (Beach, Miles, Lukes, & Vigersky, 1985). First, a standard curve of 5, 10, 20, 40 and 80 ng/ml PRL concentrations was prepared. Afterwards, 20 μl of unknown serum samples were added to the microplate, previously prepared with Anti-PRL antibody. Samples were analyzed with a microplate spectrophotometer (BIORAD 550) and absorbance was read at 450 nm wavelength. PRL concentrations were computed by linear regression itting using Sigma Plot software (data were reported in ng/ml).

Statistical analysis

PRL levels between groups were analyzed with a one way ANOVA followed by the Student-Neuman-Keuls test. Sexual behavior parameters were analyzed with repeated measures ANOVA, followed by the Student-Newman-Keuls test when the F value and statistical tests were at a 0.05 signiicance level, validating the assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances.

ResultsPRL levels in serum

Blood samples were obtained by heart puncture, after last session and PRL levels were analyzed with the ELISA kit. Plasmatic PRL levels were presented in all subjects, however, the graft group showed a significant increase (40.46±1.18 ng/ml), compared with the baseline levels obtained from intact rats (˜12 ng/ml) (Hernandez et al., 2006). Similarly, the control group showed a significant increase (19.90±1.18 ng/ml). In contrast, in the oPRL group, the increase was not significant (14.60±1.18 ng/ml), with similar results in the sham group (13.13±1.39 ng/ml)(Fig. 1) (F=8.36, p<0.01).

Figure 1.

PRL levels in serum. The figure shows the PRL levels obtained in the four groups. In the ctrl and Graft group, the increase was significant compared with data obtained from an intact rat (not used for this experiment). Data are expressed as means ±SEM. Asterisks denote statistical differences.

(0,04MB).
Effect of HP on sexual behaviorFirst mount latency

During the experiment, subjects were tested every three days (sessions 3, 6, 9, 12 and 15). The statistical analysis did not showed diferences between sessions (data not shown), but the diferences between groups were signiicative. The first mount latency increased significantly in the oPRL group compared with the control group. Similarly, the Graft group showed a significant increase in the first mount latency compared with the Sham group (Table 1) (F=3.99, p<0.05, Fig. 2).

Table 1.

Sexual behavior parameters.

Mean (X―)  Ctrl  oPRL  Sham  Graft 
First Mount Latency (seconds)  5,8±0.40  9,4±1.20*  6,3±1.40  8,5±0.74* 
Intromission Frecuency (I)  9,0±0.08  8,9±0.08  9,7±0.08  8,5±0.20 
Mount Frecuency (M)  6,4±0.19  6,7±0.18  5,6±0.66  11,9±0.22** 
First Intromission-Ejaculation (E) Latency (seconds)  237±0.10  239±0.10  217±0.35  358±0.14** 
Hit rate [I+E/I+M+E]  0,64±0.03  0.62±0.03  0,74±0.06  0,52±0.04** 

Data are presented as Means ± SEM.

*

p<0.05

**

p<0.01.

Fig. 2.

The first mount latency. Figure shows that both treatments (ovine prolactin administration and pituitary transplant), increase the first mount latency when compared with the control groups (saline injection and sham-operated animals) (F=3.99, P<0.05). Data are expressed as means ±SEM. Asterisks denote statistical differences.

(0,04MB).
Mount and Intromission Frequency

In order to demonstrate the effect of HP induction in the mount and intromission frequency, there were analyzed all sessions using a repeated measure ANOVA. The analysis showed diferences between groups but not between sessions (data not shown). The mount frequency did not increase in the oPRL group, but, in the graft group had a significant increase (F=13, p<0.01, Fig. 3A, Table 1).Conversely, no treatment afected the intromission frequency (F=1.4, p=0.24, Fig. 3B, Table 1).

Figure 3.

Graph shows results of the Mount and Intromission frequency between the different groups. We can observe that HP obtained by the pituitary transplant is enough to produce an increase in the mount frequency in 15 days (F=12.68, p<0.001) (panel A). Panel B shows results of HP on the intromission frequency. We did not observe differences between groups (F=1.41, p=0.24). Bars show mean ± SEM. Asterisks denote statistical differences.

(0,06MB).
The Intromission-ejaculation latency and Hit rate.

The oPRL administration did not produce any change in the intromission-ejaculation latency, however, the graft treatment increased this parameter when compared with the other groups (F=19, p<0.01, Fig. 4A). Similarly, the oPRL administration did not show diferences in the hit rate, but the Graft group showed a significant reduction compared with the other groups (F=13.46, p<0.01, Fig. 4B)(Table 1).

Figure 4.

Panel A shows that in the transplanted group, the First Intromission-Ejaculation Latency was increased (F= 19.6, p<0.01). This increase was associated with a significant decrease in the Hit Rate, compared with the others groups (F=13.46, p<0.01) (panel B). Data are presented as means ±SEM. Asterisks denote statistical differences.

(0,06MB).
Discussion

Several clinical studies sustained that erectile dysfunction is the main symptom of chronic hyperprolactinemia (Martinez-Jalaboyas, 2010), however, this study was designed to compare if the induction of an acute HP to sexual experienced male rats, it was able to induce an impairment of the anticipatory/consummatory phases of sexual behavior.

Table 1. Sexual behavior parameters. The table shows data oobtained in the sexual behavior parameters tested in the four groups. The first mount latency increased in the oPRL and Graft group. The intromission frequency was not afected with any treatment, while the mount frequency, increased in the graft group. In the same way, the first Intromission-Ejaculation Latency showed an increase in the graft group and the Efficiency of Copulatory Interactions (Hit rate) (I + E/M + I + E) decreased in the same group. Values are described as means ± SEM.

The results obtained showed that in both groups (oPRL and the pituitary graft), it was observed a significant increase in the first mount latency. What is more, in the graft group was showed a significant increase the mount frequency and the first intromission-ejaculation latency with a significant reduction in the hit rate. Conversely, no treatment afected the intromission frequency.

Interestingly, this effect was not observed in subjects with oPRL administration. The probable explanation, is due to the used dose (100μg/d), since with this concentration, PRL levels increase only temporarily (40 ng/ml), returning to physiological levels after a while (12-14 ng/ml). This is consistent with studies reporting negative effects on sexual behavior with the induction of a sustained Hyperprolactinemia (>100 ng/ml/d) (Martinez-Jalaboyas, 2010; Abram et al., 1998; Doherty et al., 1985; Katovich et al., 1985). For that reason, the HP induced in the graft group (40ng/ml), was enough to modify the sexual function, with significant effects on the sexual behavior parameters tested (Hernandez et al., 2007).

The plasmatic PRL levels were slightly increased in the control group, however, are not related with a pathological condition, since some studies have shown that the acute increases in prolactin levels after coitus, are related with sexual satiety, suggesting the participation of PRL in different contexts of sexual behavior, like has been reported previously (Hernandez et al., 2006; Hernandez et al., 2007).

Accumulating data suggest that the neurotransmitter DA, plays an important role in the sexual behavior regulation, results presented here, supported this hypothesis and proposed strongly that PRL is able to induce alterations in DA levels in specific brain areas associated with the male copulatory activity (Giuliano & Allard, 2001b). The precise mechanism of how DA modiies the sexual behavior has been discussed. It has been shown that some neural nuclei are associated with specific phases of sexual behavior, for example, the nucleus accumbens and medial preoptic area, are associated with anticipatory and consummatory phases of sexual behavior, for that reason, the first intromissionejaculation latency was increased in the pituitary graft group (Giuliano & Allard, 2001[a; b]). Moreover, nucleus accumbens plays a central role in the reward circuit, in which, dopamine and serotonine are regulators of desire and saciety, so, when this nucleus is impaired, the frequency of intromissions and ejaculations are decreased together with the mount latency. On the other hand, the impairment of the paraventricular nucleus only modiies the mount frequency, suggesting that, in our model, prolactin could be afecting DA levels in specific brain areas (Paredes & Agmo, 2004).

According to many studies, the maintaining of high PRL levels, is directly related with an impairment of the sexual activity (Hernandez et al., 2006; Kruger et al., 2002; Manna et al., 2001; Svare et al., 1979; Ambrosi et al., 1976), but, in our model, the effects were observed in only 15 days, suggesting that the acute increase of PRL levels in the graft group was enough to impair the sexual behavior. In the oPRL group, fluctuations in prolactin levels did not have a significant effect in the sexual parameters, which means that increases have to be constants to observe negative effects on sexual behavior (Abram et al., 1998; Doherty, Bartke, Hogan, Klemcke, & Smith, 1982).

The increase observed in the first mount latency and the first intromission-ejaculation latency in the transplanted group, support the idea that PRL could down-regulate DA levels in the CNS. In this context, it has been demonstrated that the hyperprolactinemia induced by hypothalamic tumors or antipsychotics drugs, are associated with negative effects on sexual behavior (Martinez-Jabaloyas, 2010; Cavieres, 2008).

Moreover, few studies showing negative effects in the same time used in this experiment, but the serum prolactin levels obtained were above 1000 ng/ml (Doherty, Bartke, & Smith, 1985; Doherty, Bartke, Smith, & Davis, 1985), while in our model, there were obtained PRL levels of 40ng/ml (Hernandez et al., 2006). However, our model shown clearly that with the levels obtained, the sexual behavior was afected.

Several studies have shown that the negative effects on sexual behavior (decrease of mounts, intromissions and ejaculations) (Veronesi et al., 2011), are related with a decrease in the levels of T (Paick, Yang, Kim, & Ku, 2006; Sato et al., 1997) and in DA receptors in specific brain areas (Bernichtein et al., 2010; Calarge et al., 2009; Fitzgerald & Dinan, 2008; Paredes & Agmo, 2004). The proposed mechanisms that explain this regulation is not clearly understood. The first suggested a downregulation in the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonad axis, decreasing T levels by a decrease in LH levels. The other is through a negative feedback of PRL to the dopaminergic receptors in areas related with the sexual function like the nucleus accumbens, the striatum or the preoptic area, and, in consequence, the decrease in DA release from the hypothalamus (Buvart, 2003; Kalra, Simpkins, Luttge, & Kalra, 1983), but, in our model, we do not know which areas could be afected since the hormone has to cross the hematoencephalic barrier to achieve significant concentrations in the brain. This means also that, with a intra-cerebral infusion the effects on sexual behavior are produced in a shorter time. For that reason, we believe that in the transplanted group, were observed more effects because of the constant levels obtained in this model (Giuliano F, & Allard J 2001a; Cruz-Casallas et al., 1999). Based on these studies, our results strongly suggested that T levels and the DA receptors are decreased in these animals; however we not analyzed this, being one of the limitations of the study.

Lastly, this results are interesting since a clinical view point, because the presented data demonstrated that a moderated HP is able to produce alterations on sexual behavior, reason why, PRL levels need to be analyzed in men with problems related with sexual dysfunction (Venetikou et al., 2008), and even more, in those with a sexual active life, since the moderate increase in PRL levels during the reproductive years could be related with a negative response on the sexual behavior.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have nothing to disclose.

This work was supported by CONACYT, Scholarship No. 201983 (LIP-M), 209518 (SRR), and 207930 (PBL). LIP-M, SRR and PBL were in charge of the procedures, sexual behavior recordings and analyses, and the drug treatments. LIP-M, JM and MEH were in charge of the article writing and revision. MEH is the principal in charge of this research line.

References
[Abram et al., 1998]
F. Abram, F. Linke, A. Kalfon, C. Tchovelidze, N. Chelbi, G. Arvis.
Erectile dysfunction secondary to hyperprolactinemia Apropos of 13 cases.
Progres en Urologie, 8 (1998), pp. 537-541
[Ambrosi et al., 1976]
B. Ambrosi, P. Travaglini, P. Beck-Peccoz, R. Bara, R. Elli, A. Paracchi, G. Faglia.
Effect of sulpiride-induced hyperprolactinemia on serum testosterone response to HCG in normal men.
The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 43 (1976), pp. 700-703
doi:10.1210/jcem-43-3-700
[Beach et al., 1985]
J.E. Beach, D.J. Miles, Y.G. Lukes, R.A. Vigersky.
Microplate solid-phase radioimmunoassay for rat prolactin.
Journal of Laboratory and Clinical Medicine, 105 (1985), pp. 294-298
[Ben Jonathan and LaPensee, 2008]
N. Ben Jonathan, C.R. LaPensee, E.W. Lapensee.
What can we learn from rodents about prolactin in humans?.
Endocrine Reviews, 29 (2008), pp. 1-41
doi:10.1210/er.2007-0017
[Bernichtein et al., 2010]
S. Bernichtein, P. Touraine, V. Goffin.
New concepts in prolactin biology.
Journal of Endocrinology, 206 (2010), pp. 1-11
doi:10.1677/JOE-10-0069
[Buvat, 2003]
J. Buvat.
Hyperprolactinemia and sexual function in men: a short review.
International Journal of Impotence Research, 15 (2003), pp. 373-377
doi:10.1038/sj.ijir.3901043
[Calarge et al., 2009]
C.A. Calarge, V.L. Ellingrod, L. Acion, D.D. Miller, J. Moline, M.J. Tansey, J.A. Schlechte.
Variants of the dopamine D2 receptor gene and risperidone-induced hyperprolactinemia in children and adolescents.
Pharmacogenet Genomics, 19 (2009), pp. 373-382
doi:10.1097/FPC.0b013e328329a60f
[Cruz-Casallas et al., 1999]
P.E. Cruz-Casallas, A.G. Nasello, E.E. Hucke, L.F. Felicio.
Dual modulation of male sexual behavior in rats by central prolactin: relationship with in vivo striatal dopaminergic activity.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24 (1999), pp. 681-693
doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00021-9
[Doherty et al., 1982]
P.C. Doherty, A. Bartke, M.P. Hogan, H. Klemcke, M.S. Smith.
Effects of hyperprolactinemia on copulatory behavior and testicular human chorionic gonadotropin binding in adrenalectomized rats.
Endocrinology, 111 (1982), pp. 820-826
doi:10.1210/endo-111-3-820
[Doherty et al., 1985]
P.C. Doherty, A. Bartke, M.S. Smith.
Hyperprolactinemia and male sexual behavior: effects of steroid replacement with estrogen plus dihydrotestosterone.
Physiology & Behavior, 35 (1985), pp. 99-104
doi:10.1016/0031-9384(85)90178-7
[Doherty et al., 1985]
P.C. Doherty, A. Bartke, M.S. Smith, S.L. Davis.
Increased serum prolactin levels mediate the suppressive effects of ectopic pituitary grafts on copulatory behavior in male rats.
Hormones and Behavior, 19 (1985), pp. 111-121
doi:10.1016/0018-506X(85)90012-1
[Fitzgerald and Dinan, 2008]
P. Fitzgerald, T.G. Dinan.
Prolactin and dopamine: what is the connection? A review article.
Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22 (2008), pp. 12-19
doi:10.1177/0269216307087148
[Garcia-Martinez et al., 2010]
R. Garcia-Martinez, M. Miquel, L.I. Garcia, G.A. CoriaAvila, C.A. Perez, G.E. Aranda-Abreu, J. Manzo.
Multiunit recording of the cerebellar cortex, inferior olive, and fastigial nucleus during copulation in naive and sexually experienced male rats.
Cerebellum, 9 (2010), pp. 96-102
doi:10.1007%2Fs12311-009-0148-y
[Giuliano and Allard, 2001a]
F. Giuliano, J. Allard.
Dopamine and male sexual function.
European Urology, 40 (2001), pp. 601-608
doi:10.1159/000049844
[Giuliano and Allard, 2001b]
F. Giuliano, J. Allard.
Dopamine and sexual function.
International Journal of Impotence Research, 13 (2001), pp. S18-S28
doi:10.1038/sj/ijir/3900719
[Hernandez et al., 2006]
M.E. Hernandez, A. Soto-Cid, F. Rojas, L.I. Pascual, G.E. Aranda-Abreu, R. Toledo, J. Manzo.
Prostate response to prolactin in sexually active male rats.
Reproductive Biology and Endocrinology, (2006), pp. 4-28
doi:10.1186/1477-7827-4-28
[Hernandez et al., 2007]
M.E. Hernandez, A. Soto-Cid, G.E. Aranda-Abreu, R. Diaz, F. Rojas, L.I. Garcia, J. Manzo.
A study of the prostate, androgens and sexual activity of male rats.
Reproductive Biology and Endocrinology, (2007), pp. 5-11
doi:10.1186/1477-7827-5-11
[Kalra et al., 1983]
P.S. Kalra, J.W. Simpkins, W.G. Luttge, S.P. Kalra.
Effects on male sex behavior and preoptic dopamine neurons of hyperprolactinemia induced by MtTW15 pituitary tumors.
Endocrinology, 113 (1983), pp. 2065-2071
doi:10.1210/endo-113-6-2065
[Katovich et al., 1985]
M.J. Katovich, D.F. Cameron, F.T. Murray, G.L. Gunsalus.
Alterations of testicular function induced by hyperprolactinemia in the rat.
Journals of Andrology, 6 (1985), pp. 179-189
doi:10.1002/j.1939-4640.1985.tb00834.x
[Kruger et al., 2002]
T.H. Kruger, P. Haake, U. Hartmann, M. Schedlowski, M.S. Exton.
Orgasm-induced prolactin secretion: feedback control of sexual drive?.
Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 26 (2002), pp. 31-44
[Kruger et al., 2005]
T.H. Kruger, U. Hartmann, M. Schedlowski.
Prolactinergic and dopaminergic mechanisms underlying sexual arousal and orgasm in humans.
World Journal of Urology, 23 (2005), pp. 130-138
doi:10.1007/s00345-004-0496-7
[Larsson, 1956]
K. Larsson.
Conditioning and Sexual Behavior in the male albino rat.
Acta Psychologica Gothoburgensia, 1 (1956), pp. 3-269
[Manna et al., 2001]
P.R. Manna, T. El Hefnawy, J. Kero, I.T. Huhtaniemi.
Biphasic action of prolactin in the regulation of murine Leydig tumor cell functions.
Endocrinology, 142 (2001), pp. 308-318
doi:10.1210/en.142.1.308
[Martinez-Jabaloyas et al., 2006]
J.M. Martinez-Jabaloyas, A. Queipo-Zaragoza, F. Pastor-Hernandez, M. Gil-Salom, P. Chuan-Nuez.
Testosterone levels in men with erectile dysfunction.
BJU International, 97 (2006), pp. 1278-1283
doi:10.1111/j.1464-410X. 2006.06154.x
[Molitch and Reichlin, 1982]
M.E. Molitch, S. Reichlin.
Hyperprolactinemic disorders.
Disease-a-Month, 28 (1982), pp. 6-58
doi:10.1016/0011-5029(82)90628-9
[Nasello et al., 1997]
A.G. Nasello, M.L. Vanzeler, E.H. Madureira, L.F. Felicio.
Effects of acute and long-term domperidone treatment on prolactin and gonadal hormone levels and sexual behavior of male and female rats.
Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 58 (1997), pp. 1089-1094
doi:10.1016/S0091-3057(97)00322-5
[Paick et al., 2006]
J.S. Paick, J.H. Yang, S.W. Kim, J.H. Ku.
The role of prolactin levels in the sexual activity of married men with erectile dysfunction.
BJU International, 98 (2006), pp. 1269-1273
doi:10.1111/j.1464-410X. 2006.06507.x
[Paredes and Agmo, 2004]
R.G. Paredes, A. Agmo.
Has dopamine a physiological role in the control of sexual behavior? A critical review of the evidence.
Progress in Neurobiology, 73 (2004), pp. 179-226
doi:10.1016/j. pneurobio.2004.05.001
[Sato et al., 1997]
F. Sato, H. Aoki, K. Nakamura, M. Taguchi, T. Aoki, N. Yasuda.
Suppressive effects of chronic hyperprolactinemia on penile erection and yawning following administration of apomorphine to pituitary-transplanted rats.
Journal of Andrology, 18 (1997), pp. 21-25
doi:10.1002/j.1939-4640.1997.tb01872.x
[Svare et al., 1979]
B. Svare, A. Bartke, P. Doherty, I. Mason, S.D. Michael, M.S. Smith.
Hyperprolactinemia suppresses copulatory behavior in male rats and mice.
Biology of Reproduction, 21 (1979), pp. 529-535
doi:10.1095/biolreprod21.2.529
[Veronesi et al., 2011]
M.C. Veronesi, I. De Amicis, S. Panzani, H. Kindahl, N. Govoni, M. Probo, A. Carluccio.
PGF(2 alpha), LH, testosterone, oestrone sulphate, and cortisol plasma concentrations around sexual stimulation in jackass.
THERIOGENOLOGY, 75 (2011), pp. 1489-1498
doi:10.1016/j.theriogenology.2010.12.010
Copyright © 2015. Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México
Opciones de artículo