metricas
covid
Buscar en
Neurología (English Edition)
Toda la web
Inicio Neurología (English Edition) Cognitive profile of a child with SOS1 mutation in Noonan syndrome
Información de la revista
Vol. 33. Núm. 2.
Páginas 137-138 (marzo 2018)
Vol. 33. Núm. 2.
Páginas 137-138 (marzo 2018)
Letter to the Editor
Open Access
Cognitive profile of a child with SOS1 mutation in Noonan syndrome
Perfil cognitivo de un niño con síndrome de Noonan con mutación genética SOS1
Visitas
3210
A. Martínez Planellóa,
Autor para correspondencia
, M. Sotilloa, F. Rodríguez-Santosa,b
a Departamento de Psicología Básica, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
b Equipo Específico de Discapacidad Motora, Consejería de Educación, Comunidad de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Este artículo ha recibido

Under a Creative Commons license
Información del artículo
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Texto completo
Dear Editor:

We present the neuropsychological profile of a 7-year-old boy with Noonan syndrome (NS) and SOS1 mutation.1 Studies on NS usually present a general, non-specific image of cognitive impairment; in our opinion, this is due to the limited number of studies and the small samples included, which do not accurately represent the variability of the condition, since they group together patients with different mutations.2–5 The case we present is interesting due to the scarcity of literature on the differential cognitive characteristics observed in this mutation, in comparison with the disability reported in other, more frequent mutations.

We present the case of a boy who was diagnosed with NS at the age of 4 by means of the polymerase chain reaction method. The medical history suggested normal development of motor, communication, and language skills.

We assessed his performance in several cognitive processes. His general ability index was 131, and composite score (CS) was 98 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV).

Attention. The patient obtained normal results in information processing speed (Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II [CPT-II] version 5: CS=61) and in cue-based tasks in WISC-IV (scaled score [SS]=11). He scored poorly (SS=6) in the symbol search subtest, which indicates difficulties in visual perception, confirmed by scores below the mean on the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA) (T-score=26). Variability of response time to stimuli (CPT-II: SS=51) and attentional vigilance (CS=50) were normal. Results for selective and focused attention (CPT-II) were normal, but with many omissions (CS=99), suggesting inattention. Results for response style were average (CPT-II: CS=55).

Perception. The patient obtained average results for visual discrimination (CPT-II score=51). In the perceptual reasoning subtests (WISC-IV), he scored highly in block design (SS=14) and picture concepts (SS=12), and very highly in matrix reasoning (SS=19). Visual processing (WISC-IV) scores were average in the block design+picture completion tests (CS=91). In the ITPA, he obtained lower scores on the visuomotor channel than on the auditory-vocal channel. Concept visual comprehension was very good (T-score=43, M=35) and comprehension of relationships between visual stimuli was average (T-score=34). At the automatic level, he scored poorly for visual integration (T-score=26).

Memory. Regarding working memory (WISC-IV), he obtained low-normal scores on the digit span (SS=8). He obtained a slightly below-average score for visual sequential memory (ITPA: T-score=32). Results in WISC-IV subtests for long-term memory (CS=99.6) and general information (CS=96) were excellent.

Language. Scores for verbal comprehension in the WISC-IV were high in similarities (SS=14), very high in vocabulary (SS=19), and average in the comprehension subtest (SS=11). In fluid verbal reasoning, he obtained high average scores (CS=95). Scores for lexical knowledge were high (99.6). The results on the auditory-vocal subtests in the ITPA representational level indicate an age equivalent to his age (mean=35). Story retelling (T-score=36, M=35) and verbal analogies (T-score=36) results were normal. He obtained average scores (STEN score=6) in verbal expression in the ENFEN test battery. In the automatic level of the ITPA, the patient scored highly for the knowledge and use of grammatical structures (T-score=38). He also obtained average scores (T-score=40) on word production, related to phonological awareness.

Executive functions. Scores in response inhibition and motor control (CPT-II) were average (S=42). Performance in perseveration (CPT-II) was good (CS=30); performance in visual search, attention, and cognitive flexibility was normal (STEN score=5) in basic tasks and poor in complex tasks (STEN score=1). In the ENFEN, results were very low (STEN score=2) in the planning test and extremely low in resistance to interference (STEN score=1).

Motor skills. Scores for motor skills on the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (MSCA) suggest that his performance is below that expected for his age (CS=10).

Our results are relevant for the differential characterisation of NS cognitive functioning as well as for the psychological and educational approach to patients with SOS1 mutations. Further studies on the functional variability of the different mutations associated with NS should be performed.

References
[1]
F. Lepri, A. de Luca, L. Stella, C. Rossi, G. Baldassarre, F. Pantaleoni, et al.
SOS1 mutations in Noonan syndrome: molecular spectrum, structural insights on pathogenic effects and genotype-phenotype correlations.
Hum Mutat, 32 (2011), pp. 760-772
[2]
I. Van der Burgt, G. Thoonen, N. Roosenboom, C. Assman-Hulsmans, F. Gabreels, B. Otten, et al.
Patterns of cognitive functioning in school-age children with Noonan syndrome associated with variability in phenotypic expression.
J Pediatr, 135 (1999), pp. 707-713
[3]
D.A. Lee, S. Portnoy, P. Hill, C. Gillberg, M.A. Patton.
Psychological profile of children with Noonan syndrome.
Dev Med Child Neurol, 47 (2005), pp. 35-38
[4]
E.I. Pierpont, M.E. Pierpont, N.J. Mendelsohn, A.E. Roberts, E. Tworog-Dube, M.S. Seidenberg.
Genotype differences in cognitive functioning in Noonan syndrome.
Genes Brain Behav, 8 (2009), pp. 275-282
[5]
T. Horiguchi, K. Takeshita.
Neuropsychological developmental change in a case with Noonan syndrome: longitudinal assessment.
Brain Dev, 25 (2003), pp. 291-293

Please cite this article as: Martínez Planelló A, Sotillo M, Rodríguez-Santos F. Perfil cognitivo de un niño con síndrome de Noonan con mutación genética SOS1. Neurología. 2018;33:137–138.

Copyright © 2015. Sociedad Española de Neurología
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo
es en pt

¿Es usted profesional sanitario apto para prescribir o dispensar medicamentos?

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?

Você é um profissional de saúde habilitado a prescrever ou dispensar medicamentos