covid
Buscar en
Trastornos Adictivos
Toda la web
Inicio Trastornos Adictivos Validation of a short Spanish version of the UPPS¿P impulsive behaviour scale
Información de la revista
Vol. 14. Núm. 3.
Páginas 73-78 (julio 2012)
Compartir
Compartir
Descargar PDF
Más opciones de artículo
Vol. 14. Núm. 3.
Páginas 73-78 (julio 2012)
Acceso a texto completo
Validation of a short Spanish version of the UPPS¿P impulsive behaviour scale
Validación de una versión breve de la escala de comportamiento impulsivo UPPS¿P
Visitas
21535
A.. Cándidoa, E.. Orduñaa, J.C.. Peralesa, A.. Verdejo¿Garcíab, J.. Billieuxc
a Departamento de Psicología Experimental (Learning Emotion, and Decision Research Group). Universidad de Granada. Granada. Spain.
b Departamento de Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamiento Psicológico. Universidad de Granada. Granada. Spain.
c Laboratory for Experimental Psychopathology. Psychological Sciences Research Institute. Catholic University of Louvain. Louvain-la-Neuve. Belgium.
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Tablas (2)
Table 1. Absolute fit indices of the CFAs for the short Spanish UPPS¿P for three models
Table 2. Descriptive statistics, internal consistency (Cronbach alpha coefficient), and correlations (Pearson) among the subscales of the short Spanish UPPS¿P and others variables
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Figuras (1)

Aim. Impulsivity is a multifaceted construct that has a prominent role in psychiatry and especially in addiction. The objective of the current study is to develop and validate a Spanish version of the short UPPS¿P impulsive behavior scale1, which assesses five distinct impulsivity traits (positive urgency, negative urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance, and sensation seeking).

Material and methods. One hundred and eighty¿nine participants were included in the study. Confirmatory factor analyses supported the five­factor model of the original scale.

Results. The results indicated good internal reliability. External validity was supported by specific relationships with a scale assessing emotion regulation strategies.

Conclusion. Accordingly, the short Spanish version of the UPPS¿P scale presents good psycho¿metric properties and may be considered a promising instrument for both research and clinical practice.

Keywords:
Impulsividad; UPPS; Análisis factorial confirmatorio; Psicometría; Versión española

Objetivo. La impulsividad es un constructo multifactorial con un papel central en la psico¿patología y en especial en las adicciones. El objetivo de este estudio es desarrollar y validar una versión española de la escala breve de comportamiento impulsivo UPPS¿P1, que evalúa cinco rasgos relacionados con el comportamiento impulsivo: urgencia positiva, urgencia negativa, falta de premeditación, falta de perseverancia y búsqueda de sensaciones.

Métodos. Participaron 189 sujetos que completaron la versión breve de la escala UPPS¿P. Sobre esta muestra, se llevó a cabo un análisis factorial confirmatorio que corroboró la validez de la estructura factorial de cinco dimensiones de la escala original.

Resultados. Los resultados indicaron buenos índices de fiabilidad. La validez externa de la escala queda apoyada por su asociación específica con una escala de evaluación de estrategias de regulación emocional.

Conclusión. La versión breve en español de la Escala UPPS¿P presenta buenas propiedades psicométricas y puede considerarse un instrumento prometedor para su uso en contextos de investigación y especialmente por su brevedad en contextos clínicos.

Palabras clave:
Impulsivity; UPPS; Confirmatory factor analysis; Validation; Psychometric; Spanish
Texto completo

Introduction

The construct of impulsivity, which has been included in almost all major models of personality, has been consistently related to psychopathological symptoms and problematic behaviors (e.g., dysfunctional coping strategies). It is now acknowledged that impulsivity is an umbrella construct encompassing a combination of multiple and separable dimensions2. In line with multidimensional models of impulsivity, Whiteside and Lynam3 developed the UPPS Impulsive Behavior scale, which assesses four distinct dimensions of impulsivity: urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance and sensation seeking. Urgency refers to the tendency to experience strong reactions under conditions of negative affect. Lack of premeditation is the tendency not to think of the consequences of an action before engaging in it. Lack of perseverance refers to the inability to stay focused on a task that can be long, boring or difficult. Finally, sensation seeking is twofold: a) the tendency to pursue exciting activities, and b) the willingness to participate in new experiences that can potentially be dangerous. A fifth dimension has been recently added to the UPPS model: positive urgency, defined as the proneness to act rashly when faced to intense positive affect4. These five impulsivity traits can be measured through the UPPS¿P, a 59¿item self¿reported questionnaire5. The UPPS¿P has recently been translated and validated in the Spanish language6.

The different dimensions of the UPPS¿P have been meaningfully associated with a number of dysfunctional traits and psychopathological disorders. Negative urgency has in particular been associated with cigarette craving, severity of stimulant addiction, behavioural addiction symptoms (compulsive buying, pathological gambling, cyber¿addiction), dimensional elevations on bulimic tendencies, risky sex, suicidal ideations and risk of intimate partner violence7¿15. Negative urgency, in combination with lack of premeditation, has also been associated with border¿line and cocaine dependence diagnoses16. Positive urgency has been associated with hazardous drinking, risky sex, recreational drug use and pathological gambling4,17,18. Finally, lack of perseverance has been associa ted (in combination with urgency and lack of premeditation) with insomnia, intrusive thoughts proneness, ADHD, and weight fluctuations in eating disorders19¿22. Finally, sensation seeking has been specifically associated with drug and alcohol use, gambling, delinquent acts, instrumental aggression and antisocial traits23,24.

Our aim here is to develop a short version of the UPPS¿P in Spanish. Actually, the original version of the scale is long (59 items), and a short version would be useful for both research and clinical purposes. Recently, a short¿version of the UPPS¿P scale has been successfully developed in the French language1. The French short version of the UPPS¿P was found to have a robust five factor structure (established through confirmatory factor analysis), high internal reliability, and strong test¿retest stability. Accordingly, the purpose of the current study is to explore the psychometrical properties (factor structure, internal consistency, external validity) of a comparable Spanish version of the short UPPS¿P scale. External validity will be explored through the consideration of the specific associations between impulsivity and emotion regulation strategies. Indeed, several of the impulsivity facets measured by the UPPS¿P, as well as their postulated underlying mechanism (e.g. poor inhibitory control) have been related to dysfunctional emotional regulation strategies25,26.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The sample consisted of 189 participants (111 men). The age range was between 18 and 45 years (M = 22.44 years, SD = 5.26 years). They were students from Sport Sciences and Psychology at the University of Granada (N = 39) and Optometry at the European University of Madrid (N = 150). Participants were selected through a purposive sampling procedure, by including those students who voluntarily wanted to participate. Only native or fluent Spanish speakers were retained for the study. The questionnaires were completed anonymously and participants provided informed consent. No compensation was given for participation in the study.

Instruments

Spanish version of the short UPPS¿P impulsivity scale

The Spanish short version of the UPPS¿P Scale, consists of 20 items that measure five impulsivity traits (4 items each), namely, negative urgency (items 4, 7, 12 and 17), lack of premeditation (items 1, 6, 13 and 19), lack of perseverance (items 5, 8, 11 and 16), sensation seeking (items 3, 9, 14 and 18) and positive urgency (items 2, 10, 15 and 20) (Appendix I). To develop the Spanish short UPPS¿P, we selected the 20 items of the Spanish original 59¿item UPPS¿P6 corresponding to the 20 items retained by Billieux et al1 for the French short UPPS¿P. The items are scored on a four¿point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree).

Spanish version of the emotion regulation questionnaire (ERQ)

The emotion regulation questionnaire27,28 measures two key aspects of active emotion regulation strategies, namely, suppression (i.e. trying to inhibit the outward signs of inner feelings and emotions) and reappraisal (i.e. reappraising the way a situation is experienced so as to decrease its emotional impact). Suppression can be considered a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy, whereas reappraisal is a more adaptive emotion regulation strategy27. The questionnaire is composed of ten items (5 per dimension) scored on a 7¿point Likert scale, ranging from 1 to 7, where 1 indicates "strongly disagree" and 7 "strongly agree". For the purpose of this study, we used the Spanish translation of the ERQ validated by Cabello et al28.

Statistical analyses

To determine the factor structure of the Spanish short UPPS¿P, we undertook CFAs with maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors29. We used CFA instead of exploratory factor analysis because the former allows testing specific a priori hypotheses regarding the factorial structure of the scale, which is particularly suited for translations of scales having received prior validations. In the current study, three models that consider the relationships among the five components of impulsivity were computed. The first model holds that there is a single, unitary impulsivity construct. The second model identifies five inter¿related impulsivity constructs. Indeed, prior studies conducted with the UPPS¿P has shown that the solution that best fits the data consists of five specific but inter¿correlated factors, both for the Spanish version6 and the English version4 of the scale. Nonetheless, the validation study of the Spanish and the original UPPS¿P found very high correlations, on the one hand, between lack of premeditation and lack of perseverance and, on the other hand, between positive and negative urgency30,6. Therefore, an additional model including three inter¿related factors was tested. In this model, positive and negative items were pooled together, as well as lack of premeditation and perseverance items.

Goodness of fit was tested with χ2 (a non­significant value corresponds to an acceptable fit). However, χ2 is known to increase with sample size, and some authors have noticed that it is unusual to obtain non¿significant χ2 values when performing CFAs on self¿reported questionnaires31. Consequently, in addition to χ2, two other indices that depend on a conventional cut¿off were also computed: the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). The combination of these two indices is valuable because the RMSEA is sensitive to the misspecification of the factor loadings, and the SRMR is sensitive to the misspecification of the factor covariances. An RMSEA between 0 and 0.05 indicates a good fit, and between 0.05 and 0.08 an acceptable fit. An SRMR between 0 and 0.05 indicates a good fit, and between 0.05 and 0.10 an acceptable fit32. We also reported the Comparative Fit Index (CFI). A CFI > 0.90 is generally interpreted as indicating an acceptable fit. Internal reliability of the short Spanish UPPS­P was measured with the Cronbach's alpha coefficient.

Two¿tailed Pearson's correlations (with 5% significance criterion) were used to evaluate relations between the facets of the short Spanish UPPS¿P, age, and the other self¿reported questionnaires included in the study. Pearson's point¿biserial correlation was used to evaluate the effect of gender on the impulsivity facets (women were coded as 1 and men as 2). Pairwise treatment of missing data was used.

Results

Psychometric properties of the short Spanish UPPS¿P

Of the 189 participants, 5 had one or more items missing after completion of the scale and were removed from the analyses. CFA was then computed on the 20 items of the short Spanish UPPS¿P. Three models differing in the way they consider the relationships between the five components of impulsivity were tested. Absolute fit indices of the three models tested are summarized in table 1. First, the results showed that the single¿factor model, in which all the items loaded on a unique latent factor, fits the data poorly (table 1, model 1). Therefore, our data confirm that impulsivity is not a unitary construct. Second, as in previous validation studies of the UPPS­P, the model with five distinct but inter­related factors of impulsivity had a good fit (table 1, model 2). Third, the three¿factor model regrouping, on the one hand, positive and negative urgency and, on the other hand, lack of perseverance and lack of premeditation, with sensation seeking as a separate factor, fits the data poorly (table 1, model 3).

Mean, standard deviations, internal consistency coefficient (Cronbach's alpha), and correlations between the various components of the short Spanish UPPS¿P and other variables are reported in table 2. The Cronbach alpha ranged from 0.61 to 0.81, suggesting acceptable internal consis tency for the subscales.

Correlations between the short UPPS¿P and the other measures

Table 2 describes the correlations between the various impulsivity facets with gender, age and emotion regulation strategies. No significant linkage was found between gender and the impulsivity subscales. Age was found to negatively correlate with all facets of impulsivity except negative urgency, meaning that older participants tend to be less impulsive in almost all traits measured by the UPPS¿P. Regarding the dimensions of ERQ, cognitive reappraisal is only significantly associated with sensation seeking, whereas emotion suppression is negatively related to negative urgency and positively to sensation seeking.

Discussion

This study examined the psychometric properties of a short version of the Spanish adaptation of the UPPS¿P impulsivity scale developed by Lynam et al5. Results showed that the short version of the UPPS¿P scale holds a solid theory¿driven factor structure, fitting with the theoretical model assumptions and with the psychometric features of the original version. The CFA analysis indicated that the best¿fitting model is one that assumes five specific but related factors: positive urgency, negative urgency, lack of premeditation, lack of perseverance and sensation seeking. This is consistent with the results of the validation of the short scale French UPPS¿P made by Billieux et al1, which is composed of the identical items we used in the current study. In addition, the short version of the UPPS scale showed appropriate internal consistency.

The major finding of the study was the confirmation of the UPPS model factor structure, consisting of five separate but inter¿related dimensions5. This structure has obtained both theoretical and empirical support, and in fact the different dimensions are specifically associated with a number of psychopathologies (e.g., negative urgency is the best predictor of stimulant dependence, mobile phone dependence and compulsive behaviours proneness)33¿35. Nonetheless, it is also frequent that different dimensions contribute to explain a single disorder. For example, in the case of problem gambling, it has been shown that adverse consequences resulting from gambling (e.g. financial problems, chasing behaviours) as well as the diagnosis of pathological gambler are predicted by high urgency and low premeditation, whereas high sensation seeking only predicts gambling frequency as well as the type of gambling activities privileged10,23,36,37. These findings illustrate how the psychometric evidence of five separate inter¿related dimensions suits the clinical findings in terms of significant prediction of valuable clinical constructs.

One of the main motivations for developing a short version of the original UPPS¿S scale original is to shorten the time necessary to complete the scale without altering the psychometric properties of the original scale. Based on both the good fit provided by the CFA and the internal reliability coefficients, we conclude that the UPPS¿P short scale developed here has a factor structure similar to that obtained in the original UPPS4,23, as well as with its French original and short versions1,38. This implies that our results support the use of the short UPPS¿P for meaningful time¿saving (assuming a completion time of 15 per item, completion time for the full original scale would be 15 minutes, whereas only 5 minutes are sufficient to complete the short version). This time reduction makes the use of the UPPS¿P scale in everyday clinical practice much easier and practical, considering that professionals often have limited time schedules and a considerable load of patients. In view of the translational potential of the UPPS¿P scale, its incorporation into clinical practice may benefit adequate case management and better identification of mediating or risk factors for poorer prognosis. For example, there is evidence of the usefulness of the scale to predict a range of clinical surrogate and outcome variables, such as intensity of cigarette cravings8, risk of problem drinking4, risk of intimate partner violence13 or the response to pharmacological treatment in pathological gamblers39.

In terms of external validity, specific associations were identified between impulsivity facets and emotion regulation strategies. First, results showed that participants with a high level of sensation seeking tend to use adaptive emotional regulation strategies (reappraisal) more frequently than dysfunctional regulation ones (suppression). Interestingly, Cabello et al28 found similar relationships between emotion regulation strategies and extraversion, a personality trait that, in turn, correlates with sensation seeking40. Although the specific psychological processes underlying these relationships remain to be elucidated, our results support the view that a high level of sensation seeking is not per se problematic, in contrast to the other facets of impulsivity. Second, we found that participants with a high level of negative urgency reported a lower tendency to suppress their emotions. This result could be seen as somewhat unexpected as high negative urgency has been extensively related to emotional disorders as well as with the involvement in dysfunctional emotions regulation strategies25,41. However, as people with high negative urgency failed to exert self¿control when faced to intense emotions (e.g., saying things they later regret when experiencing a strong emotion of anger), it is possible to hypothesize that they a priori assessed themselves as "people who do not keep their emotions for themselves" (although for them this is more the result of failed self¿control attempt than the result of an adaptive regulation strategy based on emotional acceptance). Our results do not contradict previous studies, as these have measured emotion regulation strategies with instruments that do not specifically include emotion suppression.

This study holds some limitations to be addressed in future studies. First, the sample was composed by university students, which might reduce generalizability. Nonetheless, as we have argued before17, community samples have often shown competence to unveil the clinical relevance of certain psychopathological traits, as has been the case of the original UPPS trait facets in relation to eating disorders or depression7,25. Second, we should note that, as mentioned by previous methodological essays42, the development of short measures may imply a relative loss of reliability in measure ment. In our case, although internal consistency indices slightly dropped with respect to the original Spanish version, they still fell within acceptable levels of psycho metric soundness and therefore, this risk is minimized.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a Junta de Andalucía (Spain) grant for the first author (reference number PB09­SEJ4752), and by a grant from the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (Spain), for the third one (Dirección General de Programas y Transferencia de Conocimiento, reference number PSI2009¿13133).

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.


Recibido el 2 de septiembre de 2012; aceptado el 20 de septiembre de 2012

* Autor para la correspondencia.

Correo electrónico:

jcesar@ugr.es

Bibliography
[1]
Billieux J, Rochat L, Ceschi G, Carré A, Offerlin¿Meyer I, Defeldre AC, et al..
Validation of a short French version of the UPPS¿..
[2]
Evenden J..
Impulsivity: a discussion of clinical and experimental findings..
J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). , 13 (1999), pp. 180­92
[3]
Whiteside SP, Lynam DR..
The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity..
Pers Indiv Differ. , 30 (2001), pp. 669‑
[4]
Cyders MA, Smith GT, Spillane NS, Fischer S, Annus AM, Peterson C..
Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: Development and validation of a measure of positive urgency..
Psychol Assess. , 9 (2007), pp. 107‑
[5]
Lynam DR, Smith GT, Whiteside SP, Cyders MA..
The UPPS¿..
[6]
Verdejo¿García A, Lozano O, Moya M, Alcázar MA, Pérez¿García M..
Psychometric properties of a Spanish version of the UPPS¿..
[7]
Anestis MD, Selby EA, Fink EL, Joiner TE..
The multifaceted role of distress tolerance in dysregulated eating behaviors..
Int J Eat Disord. , 40 (2007), pp. 718‑
[8]
Billieux J, Van der Linden M, Ceschi G..
Which dimensions of impulsivity are related to cigarette craving? Addict Behav..
, 32 (2007), pp. 1189‑
[9]
Billieux J, Gay P, Rochat L, Van der Linden M..
The role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms in problematic behaviours..
Behav Res Ther. , 48 (2010), pp. 1085‑
[10]
Billieux J, Lagrange G, Van der Linden M, Lançon C, Adida M, Jeanningros R..
Investigation of impulsivity in a sample of treatment¿..
[11]
Curcio AL, George AM..
Selected impulsivity facets with alcohol use/problems: The mediating role of drinking motives..
Addict Behav. , 36 (2011), pp. 959‑
[12]
Deckman T, DeWall CN..
Negative urgency and risky sexual behaviors: A clarification of the relationship between impulsivity and risky sexual behavior..
Pers Indiv Differ. , 51 (2011), pp. 674‑
[13]
Derefinko K, DeWall CN, Metze AV, Walsh EC, Lynam DR..
Do different facets of impulsivity predict different types of aggression? Aggressive Behavior..
, 37 (2011), pp. 223‑
[14]
Klonsky ED, May A..
Rethinking impulsivity in suicide..
, Threatening Behavior. 2010 (Suicide and Life‑), pp. 40
[15]
Verdejo¿García A, Perales JC, Pérez¿García M..
Cognitive impulsi vity in cocaine and heroin polysubstance abusers..
Addict Behav. , 32 (2007), pp. 950‑
[16]
Tragesser SL, Robinson RJ..
The role of affective instability and UPPS impulsivity in borderline personality disorder features..
J Personal Disord. , 23 (2009), pp. 370‑
[17]
Verdejo¿García A, Sánchez¿Fernández MM, Alonso¿Maroto LM, Fernández¿Calderón F, Perales JC, Lozano O, et al..
Impulsivity and executive functions in polysubstance¿..
[18]
Zapolski TCB, Cyders MA, Smith GT..
Positive urgency predicts illegal drug use and risky sexual behavior..
Psychol Addict Behav. , 23 (2009), pp. 348‑
[19]
Gay P, Schmidt RE, Van der Linden M..
Impulsivity and intrusive thoughts: Related manifestations of self­control difficulties? Cog Therapy Res..
, 35 (2011), pp. 293‑
[20]
Miller DJ, Derefinko KJ, Lynam DR, Milich R, Fillmore MT..
Impulsivity and Attention Deficit¿..
[21]
Mobbs O, Ghisletta P, Van der Linden M..
Clarifying the role of impulsivity in dietary restraint: A structural equation modeling approach..
Pers Indiv Differ. , 45 (2008), pp. 602‑
[22]
Schmidt RE, Gay P, Ghisletta P, Van der Linden M..
Linking impulsivity to dysfunctional thought control and insomnia: a structural equation model..
J Sleep Res. , 19 (2010), pp. 3‑
[23]
Smith GT, Fischer S, Cyders MA, Annus AM, Spillane NS, McCarthy, DM..
On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity¿..
, like traits. Assess. 2007 pp. 14
[24]
Whiteside SP, Lynam DR..
Understanding the role of impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse: Applica tion of the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale..
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. , 11 (2003), pp. 210‑
[25]
d''Acremont M, Van der Linden M..
How is impulsivity related to depression in adolescence? Evidence from a French validation of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire..
[26]
Mortensen JA, Rasmunssen IA, Haberg A..
Trait impulsivity in female patients with borderline personality disorder and matched controls..
Acta Neuropsychiatr. , 22 (2010), pp. 139‑
[27]
Gross JJ, John OP..
Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well¿..
, being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003 pp. 85
[28]
Cabello R, Ruiz¿Aranda D, Salguero JM, Castillo R..
Propiedades psicométricas del cuestionario ERQ de regulación emocional en una muestra española..
[29]
Muthén LK, Muthén BO..
Mplus user''s guide..
4th ed. , 2006 (Los Angeles),
[30]
Cyders MA, Smith GT..
Mood¿..
[31]
Byrne BM..
Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQS/ Windows..
, Sage (Thousand Oaks, CA), pp. 1994
[32]
Hu L­T, Bentler PM..
Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alterna¿..
, tives. Struct Equ Modeling.1999 pp. 6
[33]
Albein¿Urios N, Martínez¿González JM, Lozano O, Clark L, Verdejo¿ García A..
Comparison of impulsivity and working memory in cocaine addiction and pathological gambling: Implications for cocaine¿..
[34]
Billieux J, Rochat L, Rebetez MML, Van der Linden M..
Are all facets of impulsivity related to self¿..
[35]
Billieux J, Van der Linden M, Rochat L..
The role of impulsivity in actual and problematic use of the mobile phone..
Appl Cogn Psychol. , 22 (2008), pp. 1195‑
[36]
Bonnaire C, Bungener C, Varescon I..
Pathological gambling and sensation seeking¿..
[37]
Michalczuk R, Bowden¿Jones H, Verdejo¿García A, Clark L..
Impulsivity and cognitive distortions in pathological gamblers attending the UK National Problem Gambling Clinic: a preliminary report..
Psychol Med. , 29 (2011), pp. 1‑
[38]
Van der Linden M, d''Acremont M, Zermatten A, Jermann F, Larøi F, Willems S, et al..
A French Adaptation of the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale: Confirmatory Factor Analysis in a Sample of Undergraduate Students..
Eur J Psychol Assess. , 22 (2006), pp. 38‑
[39]
Clark L, Stokes PR, Wu K, Michalczuk R, Benecke A, Watson BJ, et al..
Striatal dopamine D(2)/D(3) receptor binding in pathological gambling is correlated with mood¿..
, related impulsi vity. Neuroimage. 2012 pp. 63
[40]
Roberti JW..
A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking..
J Res Pers. , 38 (2004), pp. 256‑
[41]
Selby EA, Anestis MD, Joiner TE..
Understanding the relation¿..
[42]
Wulfert E, Hartley J, Lee M, Wang N, Franco C, Sodano R..
Gambling screens: does shortening the time frame affect their psychometric properties? J Gamb Stud..
, 21 (2005), pp. 521‑
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo
es en pt

¿Es usted profesional sanitario apto para prescribir o dispensar medicamentos?

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?

Você é um profissional de saúde habilitado a prescrever ou dispensar medicamentos