metricas
covid
Buscar en
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology
Toda la web
Inicio International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology Alteration of hypnotic experience following transcranial electrical stimulation ...
Journal Information
Vol. 23. Issue 2.
(April - June 2023)
Share
Share
Download PDF
More article options
Visits
1712
Vol. 23. Issue 2.
(April - June 2023)
Original article
Open Access
Alteration of hypnotic experience following transcranial electrical stimulation of the left prefrontal cortex
Visits
1712
Rinaldo Livio Perria,b,
Corresponding author
rinaldo.perri@unicusano.it

Corresponding author at: Università Niccolò Cusano, Via Don Carlo Gnocchi, 3, Rome 00166, Italy
, Gloria Di Filippoa
a University Niccolò Cusano, Rome, Italy
b De Sanctis Clinical Center (CCDS), Rome, Italy
This item has received

Under a Creative Commons license
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Figures (4)
Show moreShow less
Special issue
This article is part of special issue:
Contributions from the Neurosciences of Consciousness to Clinical Psychology

Edited by: Óscar F. Gonçalves

More info
Abstract
Background

Recent studies of neurostimulation reported alteration of hypnotizability and hypnotic phenomena after inhibition of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), but the different assessments of hypnosis and the stimulation parameters still left open many questions about the role of this brain region in hypnotizability. We aimed to administer inhibitory transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the left DLPFC to observe effects of stimulation on the hypnotic experience and the feeling of agency.

Methods

a procedure of hypnotic induction with suggestions was repeated twice: before and after the unilateral cathodal tDCS over the left DLPFC. The experience was assessed through a phenomenological assessment of hypnosis and sense of agency in thirty-three participants randomly assigned to the sham or the active group.

Results

active (inhibitory) tDCS enhanced the hypnotizability by 15.4% and altered a few dimensions of consciousness such as self-awareness and absorption. No changes emerged on the feeling of agency and pass rates for suggestions.

Conclusions

tDCS reflects a promising tool to alter the hypnotic phenomena and the responsiveness to hypnotic procedures. Neurocognitive implications are discussed for the construct of hypnotizability as well as for the role of the left DLPFC in the dimensions of consciousness such as self-awareness.

Keywords:
Hypnosis
Neurostimulation
Hypnotizability
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
Consciousness
Full Text
Introduction

Hypnosis is as a subjective experience of focused attention and reduced peripheral awareness which relies on several factors such as neurobiology, motivation, expectancy, relation, attention (Dienes et al., 2012; Facco, 2021; Pekala et al., 2017; Testoni et al., 2020). Connected to hypnosis is the construct of hypnotizability, which refers to the ability to experience suggested alterations in physiology, sensations, emotions, thoughts or behavior during hypnosis (Elkins et al., 2015). The experimental hypnosis is based on the assumption that the higher the hypnotizability the more intense the response to the hypnotic suggestions, and hypnotizability was also associated to clinical outcomes of the hypnotherapeutic interventions (e.g., Thompson et al. 2019). However, these should not be considered as strict relationships as they were not always confirmed (e.g. Perri et al. 2019), and the definition of hypnotizability such as its quantification are still under discussion in the literature (Dixon & Laurence, 1992; Perri, 2022). In fact, since its birth hypnosis has been linked to the concept of suggestibility, and the main scales used to assess hypnotizability in terms of behavioral responses to the hypnotist's suggestions (see Facco 2021 for a review), while emerging evidence describe hypnosis as a multidimensional phenomenon including both behavioral and experiential components (Acunzo & Terhune, 2021).

With the aim to improve the hypnotic responsiveness a few studies attempted to enhance the hypnotizability through psychological (see Lynn 2004 for a review) and pharmacological approaches (Bryant et al., 2012; Whalley & Brooks, 2009). For example, it was shown that that subjects initially scored as low hypnotizables became highs after receiving the Carleton Skills Training Program (CSTP; Bertrand et al. 1993) and that administration of oxytocin increased the hypnotic responsiveness (Bryant et al., 2012). More recently, the non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques have been tested in this field as well. In particular, two studies (Coltheart et al., 2018; Dienes & Hutton, 2013) documented transient increases of responsiveness to hypnotic suggestions when disrupting the activity of the right or left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) through the repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). The same cortical areas have been stimulated by our group as well (Perri et al., 2022), which however adopted the transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Moreover, unlike previous studies we measured the hypnotic experience through the Phenomenological Consciousness Inventory: the Hypnotic Assessment Procedure (PCI-HAP; Pekala et al., 2010) in order to assess the variations in consciousness in addition to the canonical measures of hypnotizability. Findings showed that the cathodal (inhibitory) tDCS of the left DLPFC (lDLPFC) reduced the volitional control by 30% and enhanced the hypnotic depth by 11% on the Hypnoidal State Score (HSS) of the PCI-HAP (see methods for details). The study confirmed the key-role of the DLPFC in the hypnotic phenomena and suggested the possibility to get relevant enhancements of hypnotizability through neurostimulation. However, the use of a bilateral tDCS montage did not allow to exclude the contribution of the right DLPFC (i.e., the return electrode site) on the observed results. In fact, as it was supposed that return electrode could have opposite physiological effects when put over the cranium (for review see DaSilva et al. 2011), it was possible that the concomitant activation of the rDLPFC contributed to the results or, at the opposite, that it limited the magnitude of the effects. In order to test these alternative hypotheses and aim for stronger results, in the present study we adopted an extracephalic tDCS montage to provide unilateral stimulation of the lDLPFC. Moreover, being the agency disruption one of the core aspect of the hypnotic phenomena (Polito et al., 2013), we also decided to adopt a specific measure of agency for each of the suggestions provided. Our hypothesis was that the reduced excitability of the lDLPFC could enhance the hypnotizability and allow for a more intense response on different kinds of hypnotic suggestions. In fact, changes in the DLPFC activity during hypnosis were associated with altered executive control (for a review see Landry et al. 2017), and deactivation of the left hemisphere seems to best reflect the reduction of peripheral awareness during neutral hypnosis (Dienes & Hutton, 2013; Perri et al., 2020b). The contribution of the DLPFC in the hypnotic phenomena was also emphasized by the COLD control theory (Dienes et al., 2012; Dienes & Perner, 2007), according to which hypnosis would consist in the inaccurate ‘higher order thoughts’ (HOTs) leading to the subjects being unaware of his/her intentions in motor and cognitive actions. As a consequence, hypnosis responses would be the result of the reduced awareness of the voluntary control (COLD control) and the left DLPFC was mainly associated to this metacognitive function (Dienes & Hutton, 2013).

Materials and methodsParticipants

An a priori power analysis for the repeated measure, within-between interaction ANOVA design was done with the G*Power software (Faul et al., 2007) to determine the minimum sample size to detect an effect size (η2p) >0.14 (Cohen, 2013). The input effect size (f = 0.42) was determined using the automated direct method available in G*Power. The other parameters were the α error probability = 0.05, the power (1-β error probability) = 0.95, the number of groups = 2, the number of measurements = 2, the correlation among repeated measures = 0.50 and the ε = 1. Results of the power analysis indicated a minimum of 22 subjects with an actual power of 0.96.

Thirty-three healthy volunteers participated in this study. They were recruited from the student population at the Niccolò Cusano University. Subjects were randomly assigned to the sham (N = 18, 6 males, mean age = 23.5 ± 3.5) or the active group (N = 15, 6 males, mean age = 23.1 ± 6.4), and all of them were in their first hypnotic experience as revealed by the pre-assessment questionnaire of the PCI-HAP (see below for details). Before participating in the study, all participants gave their written informed consent. The procedures were approved by the ethical committee of the IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation (Prot. CE/2024_029) and were in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki.

Phenomenological hypnotic assessment: the PCI-HAP

Administration of the PCI-HAP includes the Pre-Assessment, the hypnotic procedure, the Post-Assessment and the PCI. The PCI is a 53-item self-reported questionnaire retrospectively assessing the phenomenological experience in reference to a specific stimulus condition during hypnosis (Pekala et al., 2010; Pekala & Kumar, 2007). Except for the imagery trial, the original protocol of the PCI-HAP includes only one hypnotic suggestion, that is the challenge suggestion of eye catalepsy. However, we have added two more items belonging to the category of motor (arm levitation) and cognitive-perceptual suggestions (taste hallucination). Overall, it is estimated that about 80% of people pass motor suggestions, about 50% challenge suggestions and 10% cognitive suggestions (Kallio & Ihamuotila, 1999; Perry et al., 1992). The intensity of the response to the hypnotic suggestions was self-rated during the post-assessment stage on a 0 (nothing) to 10 (extremely) scale (i.e., “how intense was the bitter sensation in the mouth?” and “how intense was the feeling of lightness in your arm?”).

The PCI explores the phenomenological experience through 14 minor and 12 major dimensions of consciousness, while the PCI-HAP scoring provides different domains such as the self-reported hypnotic depth (srHD) and the Hypnoidal State Score (HSS). The HSS is a measure of hypnotic depth that correlates about .60 (Forbes & Pekala, 1993; Pekala & Kumar, 1984) with scores on the Harvard Group Scale of hypnotic susceptibility (Shor & Orne, 1962). The HHS generates an estimate of Weitzenhoffer's conceptualization of “trance,” and it is based on a regression equation consisting for 10 of the PCI (sub)dimensions (see Table 1): the HSS may be the only quantifiable, phenomenological or “noetic” measure of “trance” available to date (Pekala, 2015; Pekala et al., 2017).

Table 1.

Hypnoidal State Regression Equation used to generate the hypnoidal state score, HSS. Percentages indicate relative magnitude of the coefficient. Each PCI (sub)dimension is rated on a 0-6 scale, with 0 indicating "none or little," and 6 indicating "much or complete” (From Pekala & Kumar, 1984).

PCI (Sub)Dimension  Coefficient  Relative Percentage 
Altered Experience  +.35  17% 
Altered State  +.31  15% 
Volitional Control  -.28  13% 
Self-Awareness  -.27  13% 
Rationality  +.23  11% 
Absorption  +.19  9% 
Memory  -.14  7% 
Altered Time Sense  +.13  6% 
Internal Dialogue  -.11  5% 
Altered Body Image  -.07  3% 
Sense of agency rating scale (SOARS)

The SOARS is a 10-item scale that indexes subjective disruptions of agency in hypnosis (Polito et al., 2013). For each item, participants are asked to rate their level of agreement on a 7-point Likert scale from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. SOARS has two factors: the first, Involuntariness, includes items such as “I felt that my experiences and actions were not caused by me” and reflects the experienced reduction in control over one's own actions that is characterised by an external locus of control. The second factor, Effortlessness, includes items such as “My experiences and actions occurred effortlessly” and reflects the perception of automaticity of actions. Participants answered three SOARS questionnaires: one for each of the suggestions administered during hypnosis: eye catalepsy, hand levitation, taste hallucination.

tDCS and study design

Direct current was transferred by a saline-soaked pair of surface sponge electrodes (25 cm2) and delivered by a battery-driven constant current stimulator in a randomized, sham-controlled protocol. We decided to adopt the same stimulation parameters of a previous study (see Perri et al., 2022 for more details on this choice) with a different montage in order to check for any differences of the bilateral vs unilateral stimulation of the DLPFC. In particular, we adopted a unilateral extracephalic tDCS montage with the target electrode over the left DLFPC (F3 site of the 10/20 system) and the return electrode over the right deltoid. The target electrode provided a negative current (cathodal stimulation) delivered by the software-based BrainStim stimulator (EMS srl, Bologna, Italy). For the active stimulation, the current intensity was gradually increased for 10 s at the beginning of the stimulation session (ramp up), delivered at -2.0 mA for 18 min and decreased for 10 s at the end of the session (ramp down) to diminish its perception. In the sham stimulation, the ramp up was delivered for 10 s until reaching -2.0 mA, the current was transferred for 7 s and was followed by a ramp down lasting 10 s. Then, after 18 min of no-stimulation, the ramp up-ramp down cycle was repeated at the end of the session. Potential adverse effects of tDCS were assessed by the experimenter at the end of each session using an interview inspired by Brunoni et al. (2011) questionnaire: none of the participants reported any significant adverse effect. Also, subjects were asked to guess the assigned group (active or sham) and identification was at chance level: this is not a perfect method of assessing sham blinding (Turner et al., 2021), but it should be noted that subjects were tDCS naïve and received only one stimulation, further reducing the possibility of being aware of the stimulation parameters. The simulation of the electric field cortical distribution was performed using SimNIBS 3.2 software (Thielscher et al., 2015): the present stimulation targeted the lateral portion of the left PFC probably corresponding to the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), as depicted in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Electric field modeling for the F3/right shoulder tDCS montage. Simulation was performed with SimNIBS 3.2.

(0.54MB).

For participants of both groups, the experiment started with the administration of the PCI-HAP and the SOARS (pre-stimulation condition); then, the tDCS electrodes were mounted and the stimulation was provided; when finished, the PCI-HAP and SOARS procedure was repeated (post-stimulation condition). The whole experiment lasted about 110 min.

Statistical analysis

Normal distribution of data was verified using the Shapiro-Wilk test, and all the scales of the PCI-HAP, the SOARS factors and the self-reports of the hypnotic suggestions were submitted to 2 × 2 RM-ANOVAs with Group (sham, active) and Session (pre-, post-stimulation) as independent and dependent factors, respectively. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Fisher's Least Significant Difference (LSD) test, and the effect size was calculated as partial eta squared (η2p; ≥0.01, small effect; ≥0.06, moderate effect; ≥0.14, large effect; Cohen, 2013). Correlational analyses (Pearson's r) were performed between the SOARS and the perceived intensity for each suggestion, such as between the SOARS and the HSS (measure of hypnotizability). The overall α level was fixed at 0.05.

Results

As for the main domains of the PCI-HAP, ANOVAs revealed significant interaction effects for the HSS (F1,31 = 10.8, p < 0.01, η2p = 0.26) and the self-reported hypnotic depth score (sr-HDS; F1,31 = 4.7, p < 0.5, η2p = 0.13). In particular, post hoc analysis on the HSS showed a significant difference between post-stimulation values of the active and sham group (p < 0.05), such as between the pre- (5.2±1.8) and the post-stimulation (6.0±1.4; p = 0.01) of the active group, reflecting an average increase of 15.4% in hypnotic depth. As for the sr-HDS, post hoc revealed a significant difference between post-stimulation values of active and sham group (p < 0.05). See Fig. 2 for a depiction of the results.

Fig. 2.

ANOVA on the Hypnoidal State Score (HSS; left) and self-rated Hypnotic Depth Score (sr-HDS; right). The active group increased both scores after the tDCS. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

(0.23MB).

ANOVAs on the major dimensions of the PCI showed a significant interaction effect for the (sub)dimensions of Attention (F1,31 = 4.3, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.12), Memory (F1,31 = 7, p = 0.01, η2p = 0.18) and Altered state (F1,31 = 17.2, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.36), while the Self-awareness almost reached statistical significance (F1,31 = 3.4, p = 0.06). In particular, post hoc comparisons revealed that cathodal tDCS increased Attention (p < 0.05), while it reduced Memory (p < 0.01) and Self-Awareness (p < 0.05). Altered state decreased (p < 0.001) in the post-stimulation of the control group. All the PCI major dimensions are reported in Fig. 3 for the two groups in both conditions.

Fig. 3.

(a) Radar of the major dimensions of consciousness as identified by the PCI. (b) Histograms of the subdimensions significantly affected by the experimental conditions. Values of pre- and post-stimulation are reported for the active and the sham group. *<0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

(0.45MB).

As for the minor dimensions of the PCI, significant effect of interaction emerged on the Absorption (F1,31 = 4.1, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.11) that increased as effect of the cathodal tDCS, and Time sense (F1,31 = 8, p < 0.01, η2p = 0.2) that decreased in the post-stimulation of the control group. The minor dimensions of the PCI are reported in Fig. 4 for the two groups in both conditions.

Fig. 4.

(a) Radar of the minor dimensions of consciousness as identified by the PCI. (b) Histograms of the subdimensions significantly affected by the experimental conditions. Values of pre- and post-stimulation are reported for the active and the sham group. *<0.05, **p < 0.01.

(0.38MB).

ANOVAs on the vividness of visual imagery, on the perceived intensity of the suggestions such on the SOARS tests did not reveal any effect of stimulation (all ps > 0.05). As for the correlational analyses, before the tDCS (pre-stimulation) significant results emerged between the HSS score and the involuntariness factor of the SOARS for all the suggestions rated: eye catalepsy (r = .56, p < 0.01), arm levitation (r = .55, p < 0.01) and taste hallucination (r = .44, p < 0.05). Also, the involuntariness of the SOARS correlated with the self-rated intensity of the eye catalepsy (r = .40, p = 0.05), while the effortless factor of the SOARS correlated with arm levitation (r = .42, p = 0.05) and taste hallucination (r = .41, p = 0.05). In the post-stimulation condition, all the self-rated suggestion-SOARS correlations remained significant, while the HSS correlations remained significant only with the SOARS of the eye catalepsy (r = .51, p < 0.01) and with its self-rating (r = .51, p < 0.01). See Table 2 for a summary of the correlational analysis.

Table 2.

Correlations between the main variables considered in the study (Pearson's r are reported) for the pre- and the post-stimulation conditions. HSS: hypnoidal state score; SOARS inv and SOARS eff refer to the involuntariness and the effortlessness factor of the agency test, respectively; catalepsy, levitation and hallucination are referred to the eye catalepsy, arm levitation and taste hallucination suggestions, respectively. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

  CatalepsyLevitationHallucination
    HSS  SOARS inv  SOARS eff  SOARS inv  SOARS eff  SOARS inv  SOARS eff 
Pre-stimulationHSS  .56**  .08  .55**  .20  .44*  .03 
Self-rated catalepsy  .58**  .40*  .24 
Self-rated levitation  .17  -.03  .42* 
Self-rated hallucination  .00  .32  .41* 
Post-stimulationHSS  .51**  -.14  .21  -.17  .28  .24 
Self-rated catalepsy  .51**  .71***  -.38 
Self-rated levitation  -.21  .00  .81*** 
Self-rated hallucination  .38  .36  .49** 
Discussion

Findings from the present study showed that the cathodal tDCS of the left DLPFC modified a few hypnotic phenomena and enhanced both the hypnotizability index (HSS) and the self-reported hypnotic depth (sr-HDS) of subjects selected regardless of their hypnotic abilities. At the opposite, and contrary to our hypothesis, the stimulation did not affect neither the responsiveness to the ideomotor, challenge and perceptual suggestions nor the associated feeling of agency.

The use of a retrospective instrument of phenomenological assessment (PCI-HAP) allowed us to identify the dimensions of consciousness significantly affected by the neurostimulation: they were the Attention (+11.8%), Memory (-16.8%), Self-awareness (-17.6%) and Absorption (+18.2%). Except from Attention, the other dimensions participate in the regression equation used to generate the hypnoidal state score (HSS; Pekala & Kumar, 1984) which was increased by 15.4% in the active group. The intensity and the effect size of the increase of hypnotizability further strengthened the results of a previous study (Perri et al., 2022) where a bilateral montage was adopted to stimulate the lDLPFC. However, unlike the previous investigation, the volition and the sense of agency were not affected by tDCS as revealed by the assessments of the hypnotic experience (PCI-HAP) and the response to hypnotic suggestions (SOARS). In our opinion, the reason of the unaltered sense of agency may lie in the electric field distribution of the tDCS montage. In fact, while the bilateral stimulation of the lDLPFC spread the current effects to the dorsal-medial cortical areas (for details see Perri et al., 2022) with unpredictable consequence on the right hemisphere (DaSilva et al., 2011), the extracephalic montage of the present study targeted a more focused and lateral region probably corresponding to the left middle frontal gyrus (MFG; see Fig. 1).

It is important to point out that the frontal lobe is a key-region of agency in the human brain together with parietal and subcortical regions (for a review see Haggard, 2017), and that the different portions of the frontal cortex subtend different aspects of agency. In particular, activity of the supplementary motor area (SMA) was associated with feeling of control and ownership of movements (Walsh et al., 2015), and it is recruited to predict the sensory consequences of an action such as to process agency error during its execution (Zito et al., 2020); the dorsal fronto-median cortex has been associated with explicit assignments of agency (Spengler et al., 2009), and the deactivation of the medial PFC described as a sign of the reduced engagement of the default network (DN), a brain network associated with internal attention and mind wandering reflecting the reduction of automatic thoughts in hypnosis (for a review see Landry et al. 2017); as for the DLPFC, its activation correlated with the self-rated level of hypnotic “depth” (Deeley et al., 2012), and it was associated with metacognition (Dienes & Perner, 2007), conscious judgments about the self (Miele et al., 2011) and action-selection processes (Haggard, 2017; Perri et al., 2016, 2017). It is probably not a case that the volitional control was reduced in the study where the dorsal-medial PFC was inhibited (Perri et al., 2022), while the self-awareness and the self-rated hypnotic depth are among the dimensions affected by the present stimulation which targeted lateral portions of the PFC. As a further confirmation, neurostimulation studies on agency outside of hypnosis revealed that the tDCS over the pre-SMA affected the implicit measures of agency (for a review see Haggard, 2017) while stimulation of the DLPFC altered the feeling of control over voluntary actions (Perri & Perrotta, 2021; Perrotta et al., 2021), but only when participants selected between multiple actions (for a meta-analysis see Khalighinejad et al., 2016). Moreover, it should be noted that despite agency disruption has been described as a key-aspect of the hypnotic experience (Rainville & Price, 2003), the spontaneous emergence of loss of control following a neutral hypnotic induction has received little or no evidence (Cardeña et al., 2013; Polito et al., 2014). At the opposite, the use of hypnotic suggestions targeting loss of control over one's own actions was associated with changes in the agency-related brain activities (Walsh et al., 2015). In other words, sense of agency should be intended as a dynamic multidimensional construct that varies across items and over time (Polito et al., 2014), not “switched off” by hypnotic induction but correlated with hypnotizability and responsiveness to suggestions as shown by previous (e.g., Polito et al. 2013) and the present study. In particular, we observed a positive correlation between the self-rated response to suggestions and the SOARS (mainly the effortlessness factor), as well as between the involuntariness of SOARS and the HSS, but only before tDCS; on the contrary, the HSS was not associated with the self-rated response to suggestions. In fact, the tDCS-induced increase of hypnotizability was not paralleled by an increase in either responses to suggestions or agency measures. These data confirm that there is not a causal relationship between the global index of hypnotizability (as estimated by the HSS; Pekala et al. 2017) and the changes of agency: the latter is only one of the phenomena contributing to the hypnotic experience, but is probably one of the main factors modulating the behavioural response of suggestibility. The neurostimulation of other areas of the agency brain network such as the angular gyrus (see, e.g., Khalighinejad & Haggard 2015) could clarify whether it leads to changes in the feeling of control and pass rates for different items on the traditional behavioural scales of hypnotic suggestibility. Stimulation of posterior areas could also help prevent possible confounding interpretations of changes in the PFC activity: in fact, despite the inhibition of the PFC was identified as a sign of the reduced DN activity during neutral hypnosis (for a meta-analysis see Landry et al., 2017), a greater engagement of PFC was observed when hypnotic suggestions required increased executive control (Huber et al., 2013; Perri et al., 2020a; Zahedi et al., 2017, 2019), thus suggesting the cognitive flexibility (and not the PFC suppression) as a core aspect of hypnotic abilities.

Findings from the present and the previous studies of neurostimulation corroborate the view of hypnotizability as a multidimensional construct (Woody et al., 2005) also associated with state changes in the ability to experience hypnosis. As for the neurocognitive implications, these findings confirm the key-role of the lDLPFC in the hypnotic phenomena, but assumptions of the COLD control theory cannot be fully confirmed. In fact, this model claims that hypnosis would produce a temporary reduction of awareness of one's intentions in thoughts and action (Dienes & Perner, 2007), and that this metacognitive process would be associated with the suppression of the DLPFC also leading to greater suggestibility (Dienes & Hutton, 2013). While the role of the DLPFC in the conscious awareness is undoubted (e.g., Bodovitz 2008), the specific contribution of this region in the agency-mediated alteration of suggestibility remains controversial. In fact, our inhibitory tDCS did not produce any modulation neither in the behavioural response to suggestions nor in the associated feeling of agency, while the Self-awareness dimension of consciousness was reduced. The latter was defined as the measure of “the extent to which the participant is aware of being aware of their self or whether the participant looses consciousness of himself or is not aware of being aware of himself” (Pekala and Kumar, 1984). In other terms, changes in volitional control and self-awareness may affect the subjective but not the behavioural experience for which the contribution of suggestion-related brain areas should probably be tested. For example, it could be interesting to observe if the stimulation of motor and sensory areas can enhance the responsiveness to the ideomotor and perceptual items. Moreover, because it becomes increasingly evident that the subjective and the behavioural experience reflect different components of hypnotizability (Acunzo & Terhune, 2021; Facco, 2021; Hung & Barnier, 2005; Perri et al., 2020a), it is necessary for future studies to adopt also phenomenological measures when investigating the variation of consciousness during hypnosis.

In conclusion, this study suggests the NIBS as a promising tool to modify the hypnotic phenomena. In fact, the causal approach of NIBS offers the unique opportunity to test the contribution of different cortical areas on the assessed phenomena, with the double aim to know more about the neurophysiology of consciousness and to produce modification in the interior and exterior experience. Among the limitations of this study there is the scarcity of behavioural measures of suggestibility for which the traditional assessments of hypnotizability such as the Harvard Scale (Shor & Orne, 1962) are more suited. Further, a larger sample would have made it possible to compare the tDCS effects on different classes of hypnotizability. Future studies adopting different montages and methods of stimulation (e.g., online vs offline), and recruiting subjects with high or low hypnotizability could also clarify if the increase of hypnotic responsiveness is enough to produce better responses to clinical procedures adopting hypnosis. In fact, implications of these findings might be manifold: for example, modification of hypnotic experience could be translated into better outcomes for hypnotic interventions like pain management, cognitive and emotional regulation. If so, all forms of hypnotherapy could theoretically benefit from neuroenhancement with consequent benefits for patients, and it may also be possible to broaden the audience of potential hypnotic clients by recruiting subjects who would otherwise be considered refractory to hypnosis. Moreover, even outside of hypnosis this line of research offers important implications for the cognitive and behavioural sciences, as well as for clinical applications of NIBS. In fact, NIBS are indicated as an evidence-based therapy for depression and addictions (for a review see Perrotta & Perri 2022) for which the left DLPFC is usually stimulated (for meta-analyses see Li et al. 2022; Tseng et al. 2021). According to present findings, the efficacy of this approach could lie in increasing Self-awareness, whose reduction is a core aspect of addiction (e.g., Hull 1981) and mood disorders (e.g., Pyszczynski & Greenberg 1987) also associated with motivational states (Boekaerts, 1999) and stress reactivity (Rith-Najarian et al., 2014).

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References
[Acunzo and Terhune, 2021]
D.J. Acunzo, D.B. Terhune.
A critical review of standardized measures of hypnotic suggestibility.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 69 (2021), pp. 50-71
[Bertrand et al., 1993]
L.D. Bertrand, H.J. Stam, H.L. Radtke.
The carleton skills training package for modifying hypnotic susceptibility—a replication and extension: A brief communication.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 41 (1993), pp. 6-14
[Bodovitz, 2008]
S. Bodovitz.
The neural correlate of consciousness.
Journal of Theoretical Biology, 254 (2008), pp. 594-598
[Boekaerts, 1999]
M. Boekaerts.
Metacognitive experiences and motivational state as aspects of self-awareness: Review and discussion.
European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14 (1999), pp. 571-584
[Brunoni et al., 2011]
A.R. Brunoni, J. Amadera, B. Berbel, M.S. Volz, B.G. Rizzerio, F. Fregni.
A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation.
International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 14 (2011), pp. 1133-1145
[Bryant et al., 2012]
R.A. Bryant, L. Hung, A.J. Guastella, P.B. Mitchell.
Oxytocin as a moderator of hypnotizability.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37 (2012), pp. 162-166
[Cardeña et al., 2013]
E. Cardeña, P. Jönsson, D.B. Terhune, D. Marcusson-Clavertz.
The neurophenomenology of neutral hypnosis.
[Cohen, 2013]
J. Cohen.
Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
Academic press, (2013),
[Coltheart et al., 2018]
M. Coltheart, R. Cox, P. Sowman, H. Morgan, A. Barnier, R. Langdon, E. Connaughton, L. Teichmann, N. Williams, V. Polito.
Belief, delusion, hypnosis, and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study.
[DaSilva et al., 2011]
A.F. DaSilva, M.S. Volz, M. Bikson, F. Fregni.
Electrode positioning and montage in transcranial direct current stimulation.
JoVE (Journal of Visualized Experiments), 51 (2011), pp. e2744
[Deeley et al., 2012]
Q. Deeley, D.A. Oakley, B. Toone, V. Giampietro, M.J. Brammer, S.C. Williams, P.W. Halligan.
Modulating the default mode network using hypnosis.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 60 (2012), pp. 206-228
[Dienes et al., 2012]
Z. Dienes, M. Beran, J.L. Brandl, J. Perner, J. Proust.
Is hypnotic responding the strategic relinquishment of metacognition? In Beran, M. J., Perner, J., & Proust, J. (Eds.). Oxford University Press.
Foundations of metacognition, (2012), pp. 267-278
[Dienes and Hutton, 2013]
Z. Dienes, S. Hutton.
Understanding hypnosis metacognitively: RTMS applied to left DLPFC increases hypnotic suggestibility.
[Dienes and Perner, 2007]
Z. Dienes, J. Perner.
Executive control without conscious awareness: The cold control theory of hypnosis. Oxford University Press.
In Hypnosis and Conscious States: The cognitive neuroscience perspective, (2007), pp. 293-314
[Dixon and Laurence, 1992]
Dixon, M., & Laurence, J.R. (1992). Two hundred years of hypnosis research: Questions resolved? Questions unanswered!
[Elkins et al., 2015]
G.R. Elkins, A.F. Barabasz, J.R. Council, D. Spiegel.
Advancing research and practice: The revised APA division 30 definition of hypnosis.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57 (2015), pp. 378-385
[Facco, 2021]
E. Facco.
Hypnosis and hypnotic ability between old beliefs and new evidences: An epistemological reflection.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 64 (2021), pp. 20-35
[Faul et al., 2007]
F. Faul, E. Erdfelder, A.G. Lang, A. Buchner.
G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.
Behavior Research Methods, 39 (2007), pp. 175-191
[Forbes and Pekala, 1993]
E.J. Forbes, R.J. Pekala.
Predicting hypnotic susceptibility via a phenomenological approach.
Psychological Reports, 73 (1993), pp. 1251-1256
[Haggard, 2017]
P. Haggard.
Sense of agency in the human brain.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18 (2017), pp. 196-207
[Huber et al., 2013]
A. Huber, F. Lui, C.A. Porro.
Hypnotic susceptibility modulates brain activity related to experimental placebo analgesia.
PAIN®, 154 (2013), pp. 1509-1518
[Hull, 1981]
J.G. Hull.
A self-awareness model of the causes and effects of alcohol consumption.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90 (1981), pp. 586
[Hung and Barnier, 2005]
L. Hung, A. Barnier.
Hypnotic disruptions of reading: Investigating modulation of the Stroop effect.
Proceedings of the 56th annual scientific program of the society for clinical and experimental hypnosis, charleston,
[Kallio and Ihamuotila, 1999]
S.P. Kallio, M.J. Ihamuotila.
Finnish norms for the harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, Form A.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 47 (1999), pp. 227-235
[Khalighinejad et al., 2016]
N. Khalighinejad, S. Di Costa, P. Haggard.
Endogenous action selection processes in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contribute to sense of agency: A meta-analysis of tDCS studies of ‘intentional binding.
Brain Stimulation, 9 (2016), pp. 372-379
[Khalighinejad and Haggard, 2015]
N. Khalighinejad, P. Haggard.
Modulating human sense of agency with non-invasive brain stimulation.
[Landry et al., 2017]
M. Landry, M. Lifshitz, A. Raz.
Brain correlates of hypnosis: A systematic review and meta-analytic exploration.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 81 (2017), pp. 75-98
[Li et al., 2022]
Y. Li, H.P. Li, M.X. Wu, Q.Y. Wang, X. Zeng.
Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation for post-stroke depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology, 142 (2022), pp. 1-10
[Lynn, 2004]
S.J. Lynn.
Enhancing suggestibility: The effects of compliance vs. imagery.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 47 (2004), pp. 117-128
[Miele et al., 2011]
D.B. Miele, T.D. Wager, J.P. Mitchell, J. Metcalfe.
Dissociating neural correlates of action monitoring and metacognition of agency.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23 (2011), pp. 3620-3636
[Pekala, 2015]
R.J. Pekala.
Hypnosis as a “state of consciousness”: How quantifying the mind can help us better understand hypnosis.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57 (2015), pp. 402-424
[Pekala et al., 2017]
R.J. Pekala, F. Baglio, M. Cabinio, S. Lipari, G. Baglio, L. Mendozzi, P. Cecconi, L. Pugnetti, R. Sciaky.
Hypnotism as a function of trance state effects, expectancy, and suggestibility: An Italian replication.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 65 (2017), pp. 210-240
[Pekala and Kumar, 1984]
R.J. Pekala, V.K. Kumar.
Predicting hypnotic susceptibility by a self-report phenomenological state instrument.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 27 (1984), pp. 114-121
[Pekala and Kumar, 2007]
R.J. Pekala, V.K. Kumar.
An empirical—phenomenological approach to quantifying consciousnessG. Jamieson (Ed.). Oxford University Press, New York, NY.
Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective, (2007), pp. 167-194
[Pekala et al., 2010]
R.J. Pekala, V.K. Kumar, R. Maurer, N. Elliott-Carter, E. Moon, K. Mullen.
Suggestibility, expectancy, trance state effects, and hypnotic depth: I. Implications for understanding hypnotism.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 52 (2010), pp. 275-290
[Perri, 2022]
R.L. Perri.
In medio stat virtus: The importance of studying mediums in hypnosis research.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 64 (2022), pp. 4-11
[Perri et al., 2016]
R.L. Perri, M. Berchicci, G. Lucci, D. Spinelli, F. Di Russo.
How the brain prevents a second error in a perceptual decision-making task.
Scientific Reports, 6 (2016), pp. 1-11
[Perri et al., 2020a]
R.L. Perri, V. Bianco, E. Facco, F. Di Russo.
Now you see one letter, now you see meaningless symbols: Perceptual and semantic hypnotic suggestions reduce Stroop errors through different neurocognitive mechanisms.
Frontiers in Neuroscience, 14 (2020), pp. 600083
[Perri et al., 2020b]
R.L. Perri, E. Facco, F. Quinzi, V. Bianco, M. Berchicci, F. Rossani, F. Di Russo.
Cerebral mechanisms of hypnotic hypoesthesia. An ERP investigation on the expectancy stage of perception.
Psychophysiology, 57 (2020), pp. e13657
[Perri and Perrotta, 2021]
R.L. Perri, D. Perrotta.
Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex reduces cigarette craving in not motivated to quit smokers: A randomized, sham-controlled study.
Addictive Behaviors, 120 (2021),
[Perri et al., 2022]
R.L. Perri, D. Perrotta, F. Rossani, R.J. Pekala.
Boosting the hypnotic experience. Inhibition of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex alters hypnotizability and sense of agency. A randomized, double-blind and sham-controlled tDCS study.
Behavioural Brain Research, 425 (2022),
[Perri et al., 2019]
R.L. Perri, F. Rossani, F. Di Russo.
Neuroelectric evidences of top-down hypnotic modulation associated with somatosensory processing of sensory and limbic regions.
Neuroimage, 202 (2019),
[Perri et al., 2017]
R.L. Perri, D. Spinelli, F. Di Russo.
Missing the target: The neural processing underlying the omission error.
Brain Topography, 30 (2017), pp. 352-363
[Perrotta et al., 2021]
D. Perrotta, V. Bianco, M. Berchicci, F. Quinzi, R.L. Perri.
Anodal tDCS over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex reduces Stroop errors. A comparison of different tasks and designs.
Behavioural Brain Research, 405 (2021),
[Perrotta and Perri, 2022]
D. Perrotta, R.L. Perri.
Mini-review: When neurostimulation joins cognitive-behavioral therapy. On the need of combining evidence-based treatments for addiction disorders.
Neuroscience Letters, 777 (2022), pp. 136588
[Perry et al., 1992]
Perry, C., Nadon, R., & Button, J. (1992). The measurement of hypnotic ability.
[Polito et al., 2013]
V. Polito, A.J. Barnier, E.Z. Woody.
Developing the sense of agency rating scale (SOARS): An empirical measure of agency disruption in hypnosis.
Consciousness and Cognition, 22 (2013), pp. 684-696
[Polito et al., 2014]
V. Polito, A.J. Barnier, E.Z. Woody, M.H. Connors.
Measuring agency change across the domain of hypnosis.
Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1 (2014), pp. 3
[Pyszczynski and Greenberg, 1987]
T. Pyszczynski, J. Greenberg.
Self-regulatory perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reactive depression.
Psychological Bulletin, 102 (1987), pp. 122
[Rainville and Price, 2003]
P. Rainville, D.D. Price.
Hypnosis phenomenology and the neurobiology of consciousness.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51 (2003), pp. 105-129
[Rith-Najarian et al., 2014]
L.R. Rith-Najarian, K.A. McLaughlin, M.A. Sheridan, M.K. Nock.
The biopsychosocial model of stress in adolescence: Self-awareness of performance versus stress reactivity.
[Shor and Orne, 1962]
R.E. Shor, E.C. Orne.
Manual, Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility.
Form A. Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psychologists Press, (1962),
[Spengler et al., 2009]
S. Spengler, D.Y. von Cramon, M. Brass.
Was it me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI.
Neuroimage, 46 (2009), pp. 290-298
[Testoni et al., 2020]
I. Testoni, E. Facco, L. Ronconi, F. Alemanno, M D'amico.
The role of flexibility and other factors associated with hypnotizability: A second step towards its definition.
Contemporary Hypnosis & Integrative Therapy, 34 (2020), pp. 7-24
[Thielscher et al., 2015]
A. Thielscher, A. Antunes, G.B. Saturnino.
Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: A useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS?.
Proceedings of the 37th annual international conference of the IEEE engineering in medicine and biology society (EMBC), pp. 222-225
[Thompson et al., 2019]
T. Thompson, D.B. Terhune, C. Oram, J. Sharangparni, R. Rouf, M. Solmi, N. Veronese, B. Stubbs.
The effectiveness of hypnosis for pain relief: A systematic review and meta-analysis of 85 controlled experimental trials.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 99 (2019), pp. 298-310
[Tseng et al., 2021]
Tseng, P.T., Jeng, J.S., Zeng, B.S., Stubbs, B., Carvalho, A.F., Brunoni, A.R., Su, K.P., Tu, Y.K., Wu, Y.C., & Chen, T.Y. (2021). Efficacy of non-invasive brain stimulation interventions in reducing smoking frequency in patients with nicotine dependence: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Addiction.
[Turner et al., 2021]
C. Turner, C. Jackson, G. Learmonth.
Is the “end-of-study guess” a valid measure of sham blinding during transcranial direct current stimulation?.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 53 (2021), pp. 1592-1604
[Walsh et al., 2015]
E. Walsh, D.A. Oakley, P.W. Halligan, M.A. Mehta, Q. Deeley.
The functional anatomy and connectivity of thought insertion and alien control of movement.
[Whalley and Brooks, 2009]
M.G. Whalley, G.B. Brooks.
Enhancement of suggestibility and imaginative ability with nitrous oxide.
Psychopharmacology, 203 (2009), pp. 745-752
[Woody et al., 2005]
E.Z. Woody, A.J. Barnier, K.M. McConkey.
Multiple hypnotizabilities: Differentiating the building blocks of hypnotic response.
Psychological Assessment, 17 (2005), pp. 200
[Zahedi et al., 2019]
A. Zahedi, R. Abdel Rahman, B. Stürmer, W. Sommer.
Common and specific loci of Stroop effects in vocal and manual tasks, revealed by event-related brain potentials and posthypnotic suggestions.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148 (2019), pp. 1575
[Zahedi et al., 2017]
A. Zahedi, B. Stuermer, J. Hatami, R. Rostami, W. Sommer.
Eliminating stroop effects with post-hypnotic instructions: Brain mechanisms inferred from EEG.
Neuropsychologia, 96 (2017), pp. 70-77
[Zito et al., 2020]
G.A. Zito, R. Wiest, S. Aybek.
Neural correlates of sense of agency in motor control: A neuroimaging meta-analysis.
PLoS One, 15 (2020),
Copyright © 2022. The Authors
Download PDF
Article options