metricas
covid
Buscar en
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology
Toda la web
Inicio International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology The interplay between emotion dysregulation, psychological distress, emotional e...
Información de la revista
Vol. 23. Núm. 1.
(enero - abril 2023)
Compartir
Compartir
Descargar PDF
Más opciones de artículo
Visitas
1717
Vol. 23. Núm. 1.
(enero - abril 2023)
Original article
Acceso a texto completo
The interplay between emotion dysregulation, psychological distress, emotional eating, and weight status: A path model
Visitas
1717
Anna Guerrini-Usubinia,b, Roberto Cattivellic, Alessandra Scarpab, Alessandro Musettid, Giorgia Varalloe, Christian Franceschinie, Gianluca Castelnuovoa,b,
Autor para correspondencia
a Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Psychology Research Laboratory, Milan, Italy
b Department of Psychology, Catholic University of Milan, Milan, Italy
c Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Viale Berti Pichat 5, 40127, Bologna, Italy
d Department of Humanities, Social Sciences and Cultural Industries, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
e Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Parma, Italy
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Figuras (1)
Tablas (2)
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample.
Table 2. Correlations among the study variables.
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Abstract
Background/objective

The aim of the current cross-sectional study is to assess the relations between emotion dysregulation, psychological distress, emotional eating, and BMI in a sample of Italian young adults (20-35).

Methods

A total sample of 600 participants frm the general population, were asked to fill in demographical and physical data, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale, the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale, and the Emotional Eating subscale of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire via an online anonymous survey. Relations between variables have been inspected using a path model.

Results

Results showed that emotion dysregulation was a contributor to higher levels of psychological distress [b= 0.348; SE: 0.020; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.306–0.387)] and emotional eating [b= 0.010; SE: 0.002; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.006–0.014)] which in turn, was related to higher Body Mass Index [b= 0.0574; SE: 0.145; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.286–0.863)].

Conclusions

By providing additional evidence concerning the role of emotion dysregulation for physical and psychological outcomes, the current study could inform for improving psychological interventions aimed to promote emotion regulation strategies aimed at fostering physical and psychological well-being.

Keywords:
Emotion dysregulation
Psychological distress, emotional eating
Body mass index
Obesity rehabilitation
Texto completo
Introduction

Emotion regulation is a multifaced process entailing the ability to be aware and accept experienced emotions, as well as to behave following personal goals, even when negative emotions are present while controlling impulsive behaviors (Gratz & Roemer, 2004). Emotion regulation has a crucial involvement in both physical and mental health (Gross & Muñoz, 1995). Several studies showed that whereas adapted emotion regulation strategies play a protective role against negative emotions and help people to cope with stressful life events (Bonanno, 2004), failures in emotion regulation were found to be involved in a plethora of physical and psychological problems including, among others, psychological distress, anxiety disorders, depression (Forman et al., 2009; Mennin et al., 2002), post-traumatic stress disorder (Villalta et al., 2020), borderline personality disorder (Miano et al., 2017) and sleep disturbances (Vandekerckhove & Wang, 2018). Moreover, when emotions are dysregulated, people may indulge in impulsive eating behaviors, as eating in response to negative feelings. This process is based on emotional eating.

Emotional eating is defined as “the tendency to eat in response to negative emotions” (van Strien et al., 1986). Given its association with consuming high-calorie food (Pinaquy et al., 2003b), emotional eating has been related to physical problems, including weight gain, as well as poor psychological outcomes such as distress, depression, eating disorders, and poor psychological well-being (Bentham et al., 2017; Cecchetto et al., 2021; Chu et al., 2019; Nguyen-Rodriguez et al., 2009; Spinosa et al., 2019). As a disordered eating behavior and a strategy to cope with negative feelings, emotional eating has been associated with stressful or traumatic events. Not surprisingly, since the COVID-19 outbreak rates of emotional eating increased (Echeverri-Alvarado et al., 2020; Harrington et al., 2006; Scarmozzino & Visioli, 2020; van Strien, 2018). In Italy, a recent study showed that the high level of psychological distress experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic was a contributor to determining an increase in emotional eating, through emotional dysregulation (Guerrini Usubini et al., 2021). Unfortunately, no associations between emotional eating and increased Body Mass Index (BMI), registered in the Italian population during the COVID-19 lockdown (Di Renzo et al., 2020) were assessed. However, in many studies, emotional eating has been depicted as a reason for increased BMI or even, a barrier to weight loss over time (Sainsbury et al., 2019).

For this reason, the current study builds from and extends findings outlined in previous research concerning the impact of emotion dysregulation on poor physical and psychological outcomes and, specifically, deepening the relationship between emotional dysregulation, psychological distress, emotional eating, and BMI.

Material and methodsParticipants and procedures

A total convenience sample of 600 Italian young adults of the general population aged between 20 to 35 years was recruited for the study via Internet announcements delivered on online platforms (university communication systems, forums) and social media. Participants were asked to fill in an online survey using the Microsoft Azure platform, after being informed about the research and providing written informed consent. Italians, males and females, aged between 20 and 35 were included in the study. Data were collected from December 1st, 2020 to January 31st, 2021, as part of a larger research project named “Effects of the second wave COVID-19 on general population: sleep quality and hyperconnectivity” (Scarpelli et al., 2021).

The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Center for Research and Psychological Intervention (CERIP) of the University of Messina (protocol number: 17758). All procedures were conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki and its later advancements.

Measures

All the measurements were self-reported. We collected demographical data (sex, age, nationality, work status, marital status) and physical data (weight and height). Body Mass Index was obtained using the following formula: Kg/m2. For clinical data (emotion dysregulation, psychological distress, and emotional eating), participants completed the questionnaires described below.

Emotion dysregulation

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) was administered to assess emotion dysregulation. The DERS is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 36 items, rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always), which explores the following subscales and a total score: Non-acceptance (DERS-NA), Goals (DERS-G), Impulse (DERS-I), Awareness (DERS-A), Strategies (DERS-S), and Clarity (DERS-C), which respectively concern non-acceptance of negative emotions, inability to undertake purposeful behavior when experiencing negative emotions, difficulty in controlling impulsive behavior when experiencing negative emotions, lack of awareness of one's emotions, limited access to effective emotion regulation strategies, lack of understanding of the nature of one's emotional responses. Higher scores indicate higher difficulties in emotion regulation. We used the Italian version validated by Giromini and colleagues (Giromini et al., 2012). In order to test whether the hypothesized measurement model fitted to our data, a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted using a Maximum Likelihood estimator testing the structure of the questionnaire in our sample. The goodness of fit was assessed using the following indices: Non-significant Chi-square (χ2), normed Chi-square (ranged from 2 to 5), Comparative Fit Index (CFI ranged from 90 to 95 was considered acceptable, CFI of 0.95 or greater was considered desirable), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA ranged from 0.05 to 0.08 was considered acceptable and RMSEA of 0.05 or less, was considered desirable). In our sample the original Italian validated structure of the DERS showed inappropriate fit indices: (χ2=2414,841 p<.001, χ2/df =4.17; CFI=0.85 RMSEA=0.07; 90% CI= 0.067-0.073). Based on the analysis of the factor loadings and Modification Indices (MI) we applied changes in the original structure of the questionnaire. The resulted model showed better fit indices: (χ2=1724,605 p<.001, χ2/df =3.17; CFI=0.90 RMSEA=0.06; 90% CI=0.05-0.06). All the subscales and the total score of DERS, except for the Awareness subscale, showed good to excellent psychometric properties: Non-Acceptance: α= 0.88; Goals: α= 0.84; Impulse: α= 0.87; Awareness: α=0.39; Strategies: α= 0.90; Clarity: α= 0.83; Total score: α= 0.93.

Psychological distress

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21) (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1996) was administered to measure psychological distress. It is a self-report questionnaire composed of 21 items, rated on a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 to 3, which explores three subscales and a total score: depression (DASS-D), anxiety (DASS-A), and stress (DASS-S). Higher scores indicate greater psychological distress. We used the Italian version validated by Bottesi and colleagues (Bottesi et al., 2015). In order to test whether the hypothesized measurement model fitted to our data, a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted using a Maximum Likelihood estimator testing the structure of the questionnaire in our sample. In our sample the original Italian validated structure of the DASS-21 showed good fit indices: (χ2=763,720 p<.001, χ2/df =4.12; CFI=0.92 RMSEA=0.07; 90% CI=0.06-0.07). All the subscales and the total score of DASS-21 showed good to excellent psychometric properties: Depression: α= 0.90; Anxiety: α= 0.84; Stress: α= 0.90; Total score: α= 0.90.

Emotional eating

The Emotional eating subscale of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ-EE) (van Strien et al., 1986) was administered to assess emotional eating. The DEBQ is a self-report questionnaire used to detect eating behaviors. The Emotional Eating subscale consists of 13 items, rated on a 5-step Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). Higher scores indicate a higher level of emotional eating. We used the Italian version validated by Dakanalis and colleagues (Dakanalis et al., 2013). In order to test whether the hypothesized measurement model fitted to our data, a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted using a Maximum Likelihood estimator testing the structure of the questionnaire in our sample. In our sample the original Italian validated structure of the DEBQ showed good fit indices: (χ2=1695,033 p<.001, χ2/df =3.48; CFI=0.91 RMSEA=0.06; 90% CI=0.061-0.068. The Emotional Eating subscale showed excellent psychometric properties: α= 0.95.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics were conducted to explore frequencies and percentages for categorical variables and means and standard deviations for continuous variables. The normal distribution of the variables was assessed considering indices of skewness and kurtosis of the variables. Parameters outside the limit of ± 2 for skewness and ±7 for kurtosis were considered as an index of non-normality (West et al., 1995). Pearson's correlations were performed for assessing associations between the main study variables.

We also conducted a path analysis to assess the relationships between emotional dysregulation (DERS), psychological distress (DASS-21), emotional eating (DEBQ-EE), and BMI using a Maximum Likelihood (ML) estimator.

The goodness of fit of the model was evaluated using Chi-square statistic (χ2), the Comparative Fit Indices (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Nonsignificant χ2, CFI and TLI values ≥ 0.95, SRMR values ≤ 0.08, and RMSEA values ≤ 0.06 were considered measures of good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Statistical significance was set to p<0.05.

We performed analyses using JASP [JASP Team (2020). JASP (Version 0.14.1) Computer software].

ResultsDescriptive statistics of the sample

The sample was composed of 308 (51.3%) males and 292 (48.7%) females. The mean age was 25 (SD=5.13). Most of the participants were Italians (97,3%), had a high school degree (66,7%), were single (68,7%), and employed (39%).

According to the WHO criteria, 47 participants were considered underweight (BMI< 18.5), 437 had a healthy weight (18.5 ≤BMI≥25), and 116 were overweight (BMI>25). The group of underweight participants was composed of 40 (85.1%) males and 7 (14.9%) females. The mean of age was 23.2 (SD=4.9). The group of overweight participants was composed of 37 (31.9%) males and 79 (68.1%) females. The mean of age was 26.9 (SD=4.88). Finally, the group of healthy weight participants was composed of 215 (48.9%) males, 222 (51.1%) females. The mean of age was 25.1 (SD=5.11).

The descriptive statistics of the sample are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics of the sample.

Variables  N (%)  Mean ± SD  Range 
Sex       
Male  308 (51.3%)     
Female  292 (48.7 %)     
Age (in years)    25.4± (5.13)  20-35 
BMI (Kg/m2)    22.7 ± (3.3)  14.6-38.6 
Nationality       
Italian  584 (97.3%)     
Non-Italian  16 (2.7%)     
Educational level       
Primary school  0 (0%)     
Secondary school  19 (3.16%)     
Higher school  400 (66.7%)     
Bachelor's degree  172 (28.67%)     
Master's degree  9 (1.5%)     
Marital Status       
Single  412 (68.7%)     
Married  187 (31.17%)     
Divorced  1 (0.17%)     
Work status       
Student  214 (35.67%)     
Student and employed  116 (19,33%)     
Employed  234 (39%)     
Unemployed  22 (3.67%)     
Other  14 (2.33%)     
DERS-NA    10.53±(4.78)  5-25 
DERS-G    14.23±(4.73)  5-25 
DERS-A    18.65±(3.75)  7-28 
DERS-S    12.9±(5.6)  6-30 
DERS-I    15.3±(5.95)  7-35 
DERS-C    11.67±(4.1)  5-25 
DERS total score    83.3±(21.65)  39-156 
DASS-D    6.8 ± (5.3)  0-21 
DASS-A    5.1 ± (4.4)  0-21 
DASS-S    9.3 ± (5.1)  0-21 
DASS-21 total score    21.25 ± (13.3)  0-63 
DEBQ_EE    2.15 ± (0.9)  1-5 

Note: BMI: Body Mass Index;

DERS-NA: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Non Acceptance subscale;

DERS-G: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Goals subscale;

DERS-A: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Awareness subscale;

DERS-S: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Strategies;

DERS-I: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Impulse subscale;

DERS-C: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Clarity subscale;

DASS-D: Depression Anxiety and Stress scale-Depression subscale;

DASS-A: Depression Anxiety and Stress scale-Anxiety subscale;

DASS-S: Depression Anxiety and Stress scale-Stress subscale

DEBQ-EE: Dutch Eating Behaviors Questionnaire- Emotional eating subscale.

Table 2.

Correlations among the study variables.

  DERS-NA  DERS-G  DERS-A  DERS-S  DERS-I  DERS-C  DERSTotal score  DASS-D  DASS-A  DASS-S  DASS-21Total score  DEBQ-EE  BMI 
DERS-NA                         
DERS-G  0.401***                       
DERS-A  0.212***  0.085*                     
DERS-S  0.696***  0.617***  0.229***                   
DERS-I  0.528***  0.580***  0.211***  0.694***                 
DERS-C  0.496***  0.349***  0.569***  0.553***  0.467***               
DERS Total score  0.765***  0.707***  0.464***  0.883***  0.823***  0.746***             
DASS-D  0.436***  0.422***  0.138***  0.599***  0.417***  0.455***  0.569***           
DASS-A  0.348***  0.310***  0.079  0.433***  0.385***  0.339***  0.441***  0.667***         
DASS-S  0.377***  0.408***  0.090*  0.487***  0.467***  0.340***  0.507***  0.725***  0.709***       
DASS-21 Total score  0.435***  0.429***  0.116**  0.571***  0.474***  0.425***  0.569***  0.901***  0.871***  0.910***     
DEBQ-EE  0.323***  0.307***  0.073  0.388***  0.263***  0.214***  0.365***  0.356***  0.250***  0.340***  0.356***   
BMI  0.062  −0.125**  0.113**  −0.022  −0.042  0.035  −0.005  −0.042  −0.031  −0.081*  −0.058  0.158*** 

Note: BMI: Body Mass Index; DERS-NA: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Non Acceptance subscale; DERS-G: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Goals subscale; DERS-A: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Awareness subscale; DERS-S: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Strategies; DERS-I: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Impulse subscale; DERS-C: Difficulties in Emotional Regulation Scale-Clarity subscale; DASS-D: Depression Anxiety and Stress scale-Depression subscale; DASS-A: Depression Anxiety and Stress scale-Anxiety subscale; DASS-S: Depression Anxiety and Stress scale-Stress subscale; DEBQ-EE: Dutch Eating Behaviors Questionnaire- Emotional eating subscale.

Correlations between emotion dysregulation, psychological distress, emotional eating and BMI

Correlations among the main variables of interest showed a positive and significant relationship between DERS and DASS-21. Similarly, DERS was positively and significantly correlated with DEBQ-EE, with the only exception for the subscale Awareness (DERS-A) Significant correlations were found between DASS-21 and DEBQ-EE. BMI showed significant correlations with DEBQ-EE. Pearson's coefficients are shown in table 2.

The path model

The path model was presented in Fig. 1. We tested a model in which DERS was hypothesized to be a significant predictor of DEBQ-EE and DASS-21; DASS-21 was supposed to be a predictor of DEBQ-EE. Finally, DEBQ-EE was considered a predictor of BMI.

Fig. 1.

The path for the relationship between emotional dysregulation, psychological distress, emotional eating, and BMI.

Note: BMI: Body Mass Index.

***p< 0.001.

(0.04MB).

The path model showed good indices of fit: χ2 (9.240; p=0.010), CFI (0.98), TLI (0.94), SRMR (0.04), RMSEA (0.07; 90% CI: 0.03-0.13). Specifically, DERS significantly predicted DEBQ-EE[b= 0.010; SE: 0.002; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.006–0.014)] and DASS-21[b= 0.348; SE: 0.020; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.306–0.387)] which in turn, significantly predicted DEBQ-EE[b= 0.015; SE: 0.003; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.008–0.022)]. Finally, DEBQ-EEwas the only significant contributor in predicting BMI [b= 0.0574; SE: 0.145; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.286–0.863)].

Since our sample was originally composed of underweight, normal weight, and overweight people, we have conducted additional analyses to test the same model in the three different specific subgroups. In the underweight subgroup (N=47) DERS significantly predicted DEBQ-EE[b= 0.019; SE: 0.005; p<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.009–0.030)] and total score of DASS-21 [b= 0.031; SE: 0.064; p=<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.207–0.456)], while total score of DASS-21 did not significantly predict DEBQ-EE [b = -0.010; SE: 0.010; p=<0.256; 95% BC-CI (-0.030–0.009)]. In addition, DEBQ-EE was not a significant contributor of BMI [b= 0.005; SE: 0.036; p=0.971; 95% BC-CI (-0.280–0.290)]. In the normal weight group (N=437) total score of DERS significantly predicted DEBQ-EE[b= 0.009; SE: 0.002; p<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.004–0.013)] and total score of DASS-21[b= 0.339; SE: 0.024; p<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.291–0.386)]. Total score of DASS-21 was a significant contributor of DEBQ-EE [b= 0.018; SE: 0.004; p<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.011–0.025)]. Finally, DEBQ-EE did not significantly predict BMI [b= 0.040; SE: 0.092; p<0.665; 95% BC-CI (-0.140–0.219)]. In the third group of overweight people, total score of DERS was a significant predictor of DEBQ-EE [b= 0.011; SE: 0.005; p=0.017; 95% BC-CI (0.002–0.020)] and total score of DASS-21 [b= 0.387; SE: 0.047; p<0.001; 95% BC-CI (0.294–0.479)]. Total score of DASS-21 was a significant predictor of DEBQ-EE [b= 0.015; SE: 0.007; p=0.036; 95% BC-CI (0.000973–0.030)]. Finally, DEBQ-EE did not significantly predict BMI [b= 0.166; SE: 0.251; p=0.508; 95% BC-CI (-0.325–0.657)].

Discussion

The current study was aimed to investigate the impact of emotion dysregulation on emotional eating, psychological distress, and BMI in a sample of Italian young adults. As expected, we found significant associations between all the study variables. In particular, we found significant associations between emotion dysregulation, psychological distress, emotional eating, and BMI. As expected, emotion dysregulation was found to be a significant factor in predicting levels of psychological distress and emotional eating. In addition, emotional eating was found to be a predictor of BMI. These findings were in line with previous studies, which suggested that difficulties in emotion regulation strategies were linked to greater perceived psychological distress (Abdi & Pak, 2019; Aldea & Rice, 2006; Kirwan et al., 2017; Squires et al., 2020) and emotional eating (Casagrande et al., 2020; McAtamney et al., 2021). Such results have been confirmed during the COVID-19 pandemic. According to Cecchetto and colleagues (Cecchetto et al., 2021), in Italy, there was an increase of emotional eating during lockdown that was predicted by increased levels of psychological distress, in particular anxiety and depression as well as poorer quality of relationships and quality of life.

Emotional eating was not only associated with poorer psychological outcomes but also with physical ones since it was found to be related to greater consumption of high energy-density food (Pinaquy et al., 2003a) and increased BMI (Casagrande et al., 2020). In our study, the association between emotional eating and BMI was confirmed, suggesting that the higher the level of emotional eating, the greater BMI.

Such evidence offers important clinical implications. First of all, our work exceeds the limits of previous research by exploring the link between emotion dysregulation and physical outcomes – as BMI – in addition to the psychological ones, which have been already studied. Furthermore, by elucidating the role of emotion dysregulation on physical and psychological outcomes and specifically highlighting the impact of emotion dysregulation on disordered eating, our findings could inform the development and implementation of psychological interventions aimed at promoting psychological well-being and weight loss by focusing on the promotion of emotion regulation strategies, in order to reduce emotional eating.

The present study is not free of limitations. First, the cross-sectional nature of the study cannot support causal conclusions. In addition, we involved a convenience sample of young adults from the general population in which participants have been included in the study on the basis of their willingness to participate. Even though a convenience sample is easy and affordable, it may not be representative of the population. Another limitation concerns the lack of clinical instruments aimed at assessing for possible eating disorders among participants..

Then, for our assessment, we only used self-report measures that could be affected by biases. In this regard, a recent study on emotional eating suggested that retrospective emotional eating, investigated via self-report forms is a different construct of momentary emotional eating assessed with innovative methodologies such as the Ecological Momentary Assessment (Chwyl et al., 2021). Direct, instead of retrospective measures could be free from biases such as one's current emotional state and provide an objectively assess the intended construct. Finally, quite small relations were found, suggesting that there could be additional factors we did not consider in our study that need to be addressed in future replications of the research such as alexithymia (Aldea & Rice, 2006; Kirwan et al., 2017) and interoceptive awareness (Spinosa et al., 2019). Future research should cover additional constructs, involve other measurement strategies, and test the same model in different subgroups (i.e., with eating disorders).

Conclusions

To sum up, our findings provide new additional evidence that how emotions are regulated influences eating behaviors. The results suggest emphasis in interventions to reduce emotional eating need to be placed on addressing functional emotion regulation strategies. Various psychological interventions, including mindfulness and acceptance-based interventions, have been examined for their effectiveness in emotion dysregulation. In particular, Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) includes a combination of acceptance, mindfulness, and behavior-change methods. ACT can be useful in helping people learn to be open to uncomfortable internal experiences, instead of reacting to them as in the case of emotional eating.

Funding

This work was supported by “Ricerca Corrente” funding from Italian Ministry of Health to IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano.

References
[Abdi and Pak, 2019]
R. Abdi, R. Pak.
The mediating role of emotion dysregulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the relationship between pathological personality dimensions and emotional disorders symptoms severity.
Personality and Individual Differences, 142 (2019), pp. 282-287
[Aldea and Rice, 2006]
M.A. Aldea, K.G. Rice.
The role of emotional dysregulation in perfectionism and psychological distress.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53 (2006), pp. 498-510
[Bentham et al., 2017]
J. Bentham, M. Di Cesare, V. Bilano, H. Bixby, B. Zhou.
Worldwide trends in body-mass index, underweight, overweight, and obesity from 1975 to 2016: A pooled analysis of 2416 population-based measurement studies in 128·9 million children, adolescents, and adults.
The Lancet, 390 (2017), pp. 2627-2642
[Bonanno, 2004]
G.A. Bonanno.
Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events?.
American Psychologist, 59 (2004), pp. 20-28
[Bottesi et al., 2015]
G. Bottesi, M. Ghisi, G. Altoè, E. Conforti, G. Melli, C. Sica.
The Italian version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21: Factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 60 (2015), pp. 170-181
[Casagrande et al., 2020]
M. Casagrande, I. Boncompagni, G. Forte, A. Guarino, F. Favieri.
Emotion and overeating behavior: Effects of alexithymia and emotional regulation on overweight and obesity.
Eating and Weight Disorders, 25 (2020), pp. 1333-1345
[Cecchetto et al., 2021]
C. Cecchetto, M. Aiello, C. Gentili, S. Ionta, S.A. Osimo.
Increased emotional eating during COVID-19 associated with lockdown, psychological and social distress.
[Chu et al., 2019]
D.T. Chu, N.T. Minh Nguyet, V.T. Nga, N.V. Thai Lien, D.D. Vo, N. Lien, V.T. Nhu Ngoc, L.H. Son, D.H. Le, V.B. Nga, P. Van Tu, T. Van To, L.S. Ha, Y. Tao, V.H Pham.
An update on obesity: Mental consequences and psychological interventions.
Diabetes and Metabolic Syndrome: Clinical Research and Reviews, 13 (2019), pp. 155-160
[Chwyl et al., 2021]
C. Chwyl, M.P. Berry, S.M. Manasse, E.M. Forman.
Rethinking emotional eating : Retrospective and momentary indices of emotional eating represent distinct constructs.
[Dakanalis et al., 2013]
A. Dakanalis, M.A. Zanetti, M. Clerici, F. Madeddu, G. Riva, R. Caccialanza.
Italian version of the Dutch eating behavior questionnaire. Psychometric proprieties and measurement invariance across sex, BMI-status and age.
Appetite, 71 (2013), pp. 187-195
[Di Renzo et al., 2020]
L. Di Renzo, P. Gualtieri, F. Pivari, L. Soldati, A. Attinà, G. Cinelli, G. Cinelli, C. Leggeri, G. Caparello, L. Barrea, F. Scerbo, E. Esposito, A. De Lorenzo.
Eating habits and lifestyle changes during COVID-19 lockdown: An Italian survey.
Journal of Translational Medicine, 18 (2020), pp. 1-15
[Echeverri-Alvarado et al., 2020]
B. Echeverri-Alvarado, S. Pickett, D. Gildner.
A model of post-traumatic stress symptoms on binge eating through emotion regulation difficulties and emotional eating.
[Forman et al., 2009]
E.M. Forman, M.L. Butryn, K.L. Hoffman, J.D. Herbert.
An open trial of an acceptance-based behavioral intervention for weight loss.
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16 (2009), pp. 223-235
[Giromini et al., 2012]
L. Giromini, P. Velotti, G. De Campora, L. Bonalume, G. Cesare Zavattini.
Cultural adaptation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale: Reliability and validity of an italian version.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68 (2012), pp. 989-1007
[Gratz and Roemer, 2004]
K.L. Gratz, L. Roemer.
Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26 (2004), pp. 41-54
[Gross and Muñoz, 1995]
J.J. Gross, R.F. Muñoz.
Emotion regulation and mental health.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2 (1995), pp. 151-164
[Guerrini Usubini et al., 2021]
A. Guerrini Usubini, R. Cattivelli, G. Varallo, G. Castelnuovo, E. Molinari, E.M. Giusti, G. Pietrabissa, T. Manari, M. Filosa, C. Franceschini, A. Musetti.
The relationship between psychological distress during the second wave lockdown of COVID-19 and emotional eating in Italian young adults: The mediating role of emotional dysregulation.
Journal of Personalized Medicine, 11 (2021), pp. 569
[Harrington et al., 2006]
E.F. Harrington, J.H. Crowther, H.C. Payne Henrickson, K.D. Mickelson.
The relationships among trauma, stress, ethnicity, and binge eating.
Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12 (2006), pp. 212-229
[Hu and Bentler, 1999]
L.T. Hu, P.M. Bentler.
Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives.
Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1999), pp. 1-55
[Kirwan et al., 2017]
M. Kirwan, S.M. Pickett, N.L. Jarrett.
Emotion regulation as a moderator between anxiety symptoms and insomnia symptom severity.
Psychiatry Research, 254 (2017), pp. 40-47
[Lovibond and Lovibond, 1996]
S. Lovibond, P. Lovibond.
Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales.
Psychology Foundation of Australia, (1996),
[McAtamney et al., 2021]
K. McAtamney, M. Mantzios, H. Egan, D.J. Wallis.
Emotional eating during COVID-19 in the United Kingdom: Exploring the roles of alexithymia and emotion dysregulation.
[Mennin et al., 2002]
D.S. Mennin, R.G. Heimberg, C.L. Turk, D.M. Fresco.
Applying an emotion regulation framework to integrative approaches to generalized anxiety disorder.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9 (2002), pp. 85-90
[Miano et al., 2017]
A. Miano, L. Grosselli, S. Roepke, I. Dziobek.
Emotional dysregulation in borderline personality disorder and its influence on communication behavior and feelings in romantic relationships.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 95 (2017), pp. 148-157
[Nguyen-Rodriguez et al., 2009]
S.T. Nguyen-Rodriguez, J.B. Unger, D. Spruijt-Metz.
Psychological determinants of emotional eating in adolescence.
Eating Disorders, 17 (2009), pp. 211-224
[Pinaquy et al., 2003a]
S. Pinaquy, H. Chabrol, C. Simon, J.P. Louvet, P. Barbe.
Emotional eating, alexithymia, and binge-eating disorder in obese women.
Obesity Research, 11 (2003), pp. 195-201
[Pinaquy et al., 2003b]
S. Pinaquy, H. Chabrol, C. Simon, J.P. Louvet, P. Barbe.
Emotional eating, alexithymia, and binge-eating disorder in obese women.
Obesity Research, 11 (2003), pp. 195-201
[Sainsbury et al., 2019]
K. Sainsbury, E.H. Evans, S. Pedersen, M.M. Marques, P.J. Teixeira, L. Lähteenmäki, R.J. Stubbs, B.L. Heitmann, F.F. Sniehotta.
Attribution of weight regain to emotional reasons amongst European adults with overweight and obesity who regained weight following a weight loss attempt.
Eating and Weight Disorders, 24 (2019), pp. 351-361
[Scarmozzino and Visioli, 2020]
F. Scarmozzino, F. Visioli.
Covid-19 and the subsequent lockdown modified dietary habits of almost half the population in an Italian sample.
[Scarpelli et al., 2021]
Scarpelli, S., Alfonsi, V., Gorgoni, M., Musetti, A., Filosa, M., Quattropani, M. C., Lenzo, V., Vegni, E., Borghi, L., Margherita, G., Freda, M. F., Saita, E., Cattivelli, R., Castelnuovo, G., Manari, T., Plazzi, G., Gennaro, L. De, & Franceschini, C. (2021). Dreams and nightmares during the first and second wave of the COVID - 19 infection : a longitudinal study.
[Spinosa et al., 2019]
J. Spinosa, P. Christiansen, J.M. Dickson, V. Lorenzetti, C.A. Hardman.
From socioeconomic disadvantage to obesity: the mediating role of psychological distress and emotional eating.
Obesity, 27 (2019), pp. 559-564
[Squires et al., 2020]
L.R. Squires, K.B. Hollett, J. Hesson, N. Harris.
Psychological distress, emotion dysregulation, and coping behaviour: a theoretical perspective of problematic smartphone use.
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, (2020),
[van Strien, 2018]
T. van Strien.
Causes of emotional eating and matched treatment of obesity.
Current Diabetes Reports, (2018), pp. 18
[van Strien et al., 1986]
T. van Strien, J.E.R. Frijters, G.P.A. Bergers, P.B. Defares.
The Dutch eating behavior questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5 (1986), pp. 295-315
[Vandekerckhove and Wang, 2018]
M. Vandekerckhove, Y.L. Wang.
Emotion, emotion regulation and sleep: An intimate relationship.
AIMS Neuroscience, 5 (2018), pp. 1-17
[Villalta et al., 2020]
L. Villalta, S. Khadr, K.C. Chua, T. Kramer, V. Clarke, R.M. Viner, A. Stringaris, P. Smith.
Complex post-traumatic stress symptoms in female adolescents: the role of emotion dysregulation in impairment and trauma exposure after an acute sexual assault.
European Journal of Psychotraumatology, (2020), pp. 11
[West et al., 1995]
S.G. West, J.F. Finch, P.J. Curran.
Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies.
Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications, pp. 56-75
Copyright © 2022. The Author(s)
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo