metricas
covid
Buscar en
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology
Toda la web
Inicio International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology Validation of the multidimensional model of the subjective orgasm experience in ...
Información de la revista
Vol. 24. Núm. 2.
(abril - junio 2024)
Compartir
Compartir
Descargar PDF
Más opciones de artículo
Visitas
405
Vol. 24. Núm. 2.
(abril - junio 2024)
Acceso a texto completo
Validation of the multidimensional model of the subjective orgasm experience in the context of same-sex relationships
Visitas
405
Pablo Mangasa, Juan Carlos Sierraa,
Autor para correspondencia
jcsierra@ugr.es

Corresponding author at: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, 18011, Campus Universitario de Cartuja s/n. Granada, Spain.
, Ana Álvarez-Muelasa, Oscar Cervillaa, Gracia M. Sánchez-Péreza, Reina Granadosb
a Mind, Brain, and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Spain
b Department of Nursey, Health Sciences Faculty, University of Granada, Spain
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Figuras (1)
Tablas (3)
Table 1. Sociodemographic and sexual history characteristics of the participants.
Table 2. Correlations between the subjective orgasm experience dimensions (Affective, Sensory, Intimacy, and Rewards) and different sexual arousal measures.
Table 3. Multiple regression models for subjective orgasm experience dimensions.
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Abstract
Background/objective

The Multidimensional Model of the Subjective Orgasm Experience (MMSOE) has been validated in the context of heterosexual relationships, with no evidence in the context of same-sex relationships. This study aims to examine the association of its dimensions (Affective, Intimacy, Sensory, and Rewards) with the propensity for sexual excitation, rating of sexual arousal, rating of genital sensations, and genital response.

Method

Sixty-eight young adults (34 males and 34 females) who were sexually active with people of the same sex participated in a laboratory task in which they viewed content-neutral and sexually explicit gay films. Regression models were conducted to explain the dimensions of MMSOE from measures of sexual arousal.

Results

In males, the rating of sexual arousal explained the Sensory dimension, while the genital response explained the Affective dimension, with sexual arousal as a state gaining more prominence. In females, however, the propensity for sexual excitation explained the Sensory dimension, with more salience of sexual arousal as a trait.

Conclusions

The MMSOE is shown to be a valid theoretical framework for the study of orgasmic experience in the context of same-sex relationships, with clear implications for clinical practice.

Keywords:
Multidimensional Model of the Subjective Orgasm Experience
Same-sex relationships
Sexual excitation
Subjective sexual arousal
Genital response
Texto completo

From a psychophysiological point of view, orgasm can be understood as the climax of sexual pleasure, characterized by rhythmic contractions of the perineal organs and changes in the cardiovascular and respiratory systems, combined with the release of sexual tension (Schiavi & Segraves, 1995). The sensation of pleasure associated with orgasm creates an altered state of consciousness, producing well-being and satisfaction (Meston et al., 2004). This indicator of pleasure and sexual health (Kontula & Miettinen, 2016) represents a broader phenomenon than ejaculation, also linked to emotional reactions (Lorentzen, 2007). In addition, orgasm has also been understood as a sociocultural event and as a phenomenon that must also be understood through its socially embedded meaning (Frith, 2015). An underexplored dimension of orgasm is its subjective experience, which refers to its perception and evaluation at a psychological level (Arcos-Romero & Sierra, 2018; Mah & Binik, 2001). However, in recent years a trend is observed to put the focus on the subjective orgasm experience (SOE, henceforth; e.g., Arcos-Romero & Sierra, 2023; Sierra, Muñoz-García & Mangas, 2024), as opposed to its traditional study in terms of frequency and/or difficulty in obtaining it (e.g., Bancroft et al., 2003; Simons & Carey, 2001; Wade et al., 2005), which left aside the psychological assessment of its intensity.

SOE constitutes an important dimension of sexual functioning given its association with different manifestations of sexual health, such as, for example, sexual satisfaction (Arcos-Romero & Sierra, 2020; Mangas, Sierra & Granados, 2024). Few studies have examined SOE in people who fall outside the traditional heterosexual schema (e.g., Mangas, Granados et al., 2022; Muñoz-García et al., 2023). In this line, Mangas, Granados et al. (2022) validated the Spanish version of the Orgasm Rating Scale (ORS; Arcos-Romero et al., 2018) -a scale that allows assessing SOE- in gay and lesbian population, showing ability to discriminate between people with and without orgasmic difficulties. For their part, Muñoz-García et al. (2023) reported that heterosexual people value, in general terms, their orgasms more intensely than gay people. In the present study we decided to focus on this collective, since there is recent evidence that SOE has differential manifestations between hetero and gay populations (e.g., Pérez-Amorós et al., 2024; Sierra, Muñoz-García & Mangas, 2024).

The Multidimensional Model of Subjective Orgasm Experience (MMSOE; Mah & Binik, 2001) conceptualized SOE in sensory, evaluative, and affective terms. Arcos-Romero et al. (2018), in the validation of the Orgasm Rating Scale in Spanish population, supported in the context of heterosexual relationships this multidimensionality by proposing four distinctive dimensions of SOE: Affective (encompassing the emotions experienced during orgasm), Sensory (relating to the perception of physiological changes), Intimacy (reflecting the intimate aspect of the orgasmic experience), and Rewards (denoting the consequences derived from orgasm). Subsequently, Arcos-Romero et al. (2019), with the purpose of giving validity to this multidimensional model of SOE, observed that, in males, the propensity for sexual excitation correlated in a positive sense with the Affective, Sensory, and Rewards dimensions of SOE, while the genital response did so with the Intimacy dimension; in females, it was the subjective sexual arousal experienced to visual sexual stimuli that correlated with the Sensory dimension.

The importance of the MMSOE lies in the fact that it is the only consolidated theoretical model that conceptualizes orgasm based exclusively on the subjective appraisal of it. The semantic richness derived from the adjectives that compose it makes it possible to attend and assess the uniqueness of this experience. This model has notable implications for sexual well-being and the promotion of sexual health, highlighting the associations of SOE with sexual satisfaction (Arcos-Romero & Sierra, 2020; Mangas, Sierra & Granados, 2024). This clinical applicability stems from the fact that the model allows, among other aspects, to establish differences/similarities between sexes and to be used as a clinical tool (Mah & Binik, 2001). Despite being a classical model, it continues to be a theoretical reference in the study of orgasm, with the adjectives that compose it having also qualitative evidence (Mangas, da Silva Alves et al., 2024).

Moreover, sexual arousal is defined as an emotional or motivational condition that can be activated by internal and/or external stimuli, manifesting itself both physiologically and psychologically (Bancroft & Janssen, 2000; Janssen, 2011). In the present work we consider, on one side, sexual arousal as a state, being evaluated objectively (i.e., genital response) and subjectively (i.e., rating of sexual arousal and rating of genital sensations). This dimension refers to how sexual arousal is experienced in a transitory and present-focused way. In addition, we also consider sexual arousal as a trait (i.e., propensity for sexual excitation), referring to its experience in a more stable and dispositional way.

Taking as a reference the approach of Arcos-Romero et al. (2019), we consider that an adequate procedure to provide validity evidence to the MMSOE is to relate its dimensions to different measures of sexual arousal (Brody et al., 2013; Paterson et al., 2014), taking into account the association of these two dimensions of human sexual response (Arcos-Romero & Sierra, 2020; Cervilla et al., 2023; Mangas, Granados et al., 2022). We believe that certain constructs of sexuality (e.g., the subjective orgasm experience) are susceptible to receiving evidence of validity by relating them to objective or psychophysiological measures, being the genital response an excellent example of this (Korff & Geer, 1983; Sierra, Arcos-Romero et al., 2023).

The above precedents provided evidence of validity to the MMSOE only in the context of heterosexual relationships (Arcos-Romero et al., 2019), however this study extends the proposal of Arcos-Romero et al. (2018) to the context of same-sex relationships. To this end, the capacity to explain the four dimensions of the MMSOE (i.e., Affective, Sensory, Intimacy, and Rewards) by the propensity for sexual excitation, subjective sexual arousal, and the genital response experienced when viewing films -that include two people of the same sex having sexual relationships- will be examined in people who have relationships with same-sex partners. In line with the results of Arcos-Romero et al. (2019), sexual excitation/arousal is expected to explain part of the variance in SOE dimensions.

MethodParticipants

Sixty-eight Spanish cisgender young adults (34 males and 34 females) aged 18 to 30 years participated. Their sexual relationships were with people of the same sex. Sociodemographic and sexual history characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1. Inclusion criteria considered were having sexual relationships with people of the same sex and having had orgasmic experiences in such relationships in the last three months. The exclusion criteria were: (a) having medical problems, sexual dysfunctions, and/or psychological disorders; (b) taking medication that could interfere with sexual functioning (e.g., anxiolytics, antihypertensives, antidepressants); (c) drugs/alcohol abuse; and (d) history of sexual abuse.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic and sexual history characteristics of the participants.

Males (n = 34)Females (n = 34)   
Range  M (SDRange  M (SDt / χ2  d / V 
Age (years)  18–30  22.24 (2.83)  18–26  20.94 (2.13)  2.13*  0.52 
  n (%)    n (%)       
Education level          0.16  0.05 
Graduate degree  31 (91.2)    30 (88.2)       
Postgraduate degree  3 (8.8)    4 (11.8)       
  M (SD)    M (SD)       
Age of first sexual relationship (in years)  16.91 (2.25)    15.62 (1.26)    2.93⁎⁎  0.71 
  n (%)    n (%)       
Current relationship          13.53⁎⁎⁎  0.45 
Yes  12 (35.3)    27 (79.4)       
No  22 (64.7)    7 (20.6)       
  M (SD)    M (SD)    1.51  0.48 
Relationship length (in months)  29.58 (25.30)    19.33 (16.67)       
  Mₑ  M (SD)  Mₑ  M (SD)     
Number of lifetime sexual partners  10  19.38 (23.31)  7.41 (8.89)  2.80⁎⁎⁎  0.68 

Note. Mₑ = median; M = mean; SD = standard deviation.

p < .05,

⁎⁎

p < .01,

⁎⁎⁎

p < .001.

Instruments

Sociodemographic and Sexual History Questionnaire. Collected information on sex, age, nationality, sexual relationships, medical/psychological/sexological problems, pharmacological treatment, drug and alcohol use, and sexual abuse and victimization.

Spanish version of the Orgasm Rating Scale (ORS; Mah & Binik, 2020) by Arcos-Romero et al. (2018). This measure assessed SOE in the context of couple sexual relationships employing 25 adjectives distributed in four factors: Affective (e.g., “Blissful”), Sensory (e.g., “Pulsating”), Intimacy (e.g., “Tender”), and Rewards (e.g., “Relaxing”). It used a 6-point Likert scale to quantify how well each of the 25 adjectives described the most recent orgasmic experience experienced in the context of sexual relationships, from 0 (does not describe it at all) to 5 (describes it perfectly) so that the higher the score, the greater the subjective orgasm intensity. It had good reliability and validity indicators in its adaptation to Spanish heterosexual (Arcos-Romero et al., 2018) and gay (Mangas, Granados et al., 2022) populations. In this study, McDonaldʼs omega ranged between .80 (Affective) and .94 (Sensory), and Cronbachʼs alpha ranged between .79 (Affective) and .94 (Sensory).

Spanish version of the Sexual Inhibition/Excitation Scales-Short Form (SIS/SES-SF; Carpenter et al., 2011) by Moyano and Sierra (2014). It is composed of 14 items distributed in three subscales, which evaluate the propensity to sexual excitation/inhibition: Sexual excitation (SES; e.g., “When a sexually attractive stranger accidentally touches me, I easily become aroused”), Sexual inhibition due to threat of performance failure (SIS1; e.g., “I cannot get aroused unless I focus exclusively on sexual stimulation”), and Sexual inhibition due to threat of performance consequences (SIS2; e.g., “If I am having sex in a secluded outdoor place and I think that someone is nearby, I am not likely to get very aroused”). It used a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree). All scores were inverted, so that higher scores show a higher propensity for sexual excitation/inhibition. It had good internal consistency values, with a Cronbachʼs alpha between .66 and .84 in young people (Sierra, Cervilla et al., 2024). Only the SES subscale was considered in this study, whose McDonaldʼs omega was .64 and Cronbachʼs alpha was .62.

Spanish version of the Rating of Sexual Arousal (RSA; Mosher, 2011) by Sierra et al. (2017). It assessed self-reported sexual arousal in a specific situation, such as when viewing sexually explicit material. Its five items (overall level of sexual arousal, intensity of genital sensations, sensations of warmth experienced, non-genital physical sensations, and level of sexual concentration) are answered on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (none) to 7 (extremely). It presents adequate internal consistency (Cronbachʼs alpha of .90) and adequate evidence of validity (Sierra et al., 2017, 2019), with the McDonaldʼs omega and Cronbachʼs alpha in this study being .92.

Spanish version of the Rating of Genital Sensations (RGS; Mosher, 2011) by Sierra et al. (2017). It assessed the self-reported genital sensations experienced by the person in response to explicit sexual stimuli. It consists of a list of 11 descriptions from “No genital sensation” to “Multiple orgasm: repeated orgasmic experiences in a single sexual episode”. Its measures show adequate evidence of validity (Sierra et al., 2017, 2019).

Materials

BIOPAC Model MP150 polygraph with 16 channels (BIOPAC Systems Inc., Goleta, CA, USA), with the AcqKnowledge 5.0 software. These were used for the acquisition and processing of psychophysiological data. Two different modules were used depending on the personʼs genitalia, in order to measure the genital response: a penile plethysmograph (Biopac amplifier DA100C and indium/gallium plethysmograph sensors) and a vaginal photoplethysmography (Biopac amplifier PPG100C and vaginal transducers). These two procedures represent the most common measures of genital vasocongestion. On the one hand, the penile plethysmograph consists of placing an extensometer around the penis to evaluate changes in penile tumescence (Janssen & Prause, 2016; Kuban et al., 1999). On the other hand, the vaginal photoplethysmograph is a tampon-sized intravaginal device that emits light and records backscattered light to assess changes in vaginal pulse amplitude in the tissues surrounding the vaginal canal (Janssen & Prause, 2016; Prause & Janssen, 2005). We calculated the genital response from the difference between the scores of the explicit sexual stimulus and the baseline stimulus, in line with previous studies of this nature (Álvarez-Muelas et al., 2022; Arcos-Romero et al., 2019; Granados et al., 2020).

Visual stimuli. Neutral videos (nature documentaries) were used to establish the baseline. As sexually explicit stimuli, videos showing two people of the same sex having sexual relationships were used (two videos with gay relationships for the male participants, and two videos with lesbian relationships for the female participants). All videos had a duration of three minutes and were previously validated at the Human Sexuality Laboratory at the University of Granada, showing capacity to generate sexual excitation, both subjectively (Mangas, Cervilla et al., 2022) and physiologically (Mangas & Sierra, 2021).

Procedure

Through different communication channels of the University of Granada, young Spanish people who had sexual relationships with people of the same sex were invited to participate voluntarily. The recruitment of participants was carried out between November 2021 and March 2023. In the first phase, those interested in participating, once they had accepted an informed consent, completed a battery of screening instruments to ensure compliance with the inclusion/exclusion criteria. Those subjects who met the inclusion/exclusion criteria were referred to the Sexuality Laboratory. Females were not screened during menstruation, and all participants were required to abstain from caffeine, alcohol, and sexual encounters with others or alone for 24 h before the experiment. Of all the participants who responded to the screening phase and potentially met the inclusion criteria for participation in this study, 84 (55.26 % of total) were finally excluded. The main reasons for exclusion, in order of frequency, were the following: (1) inability to contact them for the second in-person phase of the study, (2) appointment cancellation or withdrawal, (3) mechanical or procedural errors in both the recording devices and the signal representation software, (4) excess of signals in any of the counterbalance sequences, and (5) voluntary abandonment. This study was previously approved by the Ethical Committee of Human Research of the University of Granada (reference 2308/CEIH/2021).

In the laboratory, before the start of the experimental task, participants accepted a second informed consent. Anonymity and confidentiality were guaranteed at all times. After this, an experimented researcher explained the functioning, placement, and adjustment of the genital response recording devices. Subsequently, the researcher retired to the control room to ensure a correct recording of the psychophysiological signal, and five minutes of adaptation were left before starting the experiment. The light and temperature of the experimental room were kept stable.

The experimental sequence consisted of viewing: (a) neutral baseline video; (b) neutral video 1 and sexual video 1; and (c) neutral video 2 and sexual video 2. To avoid order effects, the videos were randomized and counterbalanced (Álvarez-Muelas et al., 2022; Cervilla et al., 2024; Granados et al., 2020). After the neutral baseline video and each sexual video, participants responded to the Rating of Sexual Arousal and Rating of Genital Sensations. See Fig. 1. At the end of the experiment, participants were gifted with sexual health promotion kits containing single-dose lubricants and condoms.

Fig. 1.

Graphical Experimental Procedure and Position of the Participant.

(0.32MB).
Data analysis

The necessary sample size calculation was conducted using the software G*Power program (Faul et al., 2007) for regression models. Considering a power calculation (α = .05, power = 0.80, d = 0.45, number of predictors = 4) a minimum of 32 participants per sex was determined. Pearson correlations were used to associate the four dimensions of SOE and the different measures of sexual arousal. Additionally, multiple regression models were conducted by the stepwise method to explain the SOE dimensions from the arousal measures separately in males and females. The predictor variables were divided into two blocks: (1) propensity for sexual excitation, and (2) rating of sexual arousal, rating of genital sensations, and genital response.

ResultsBivariate correlations

In the male sample, statistically significant correlations were obtained, in a positive way, between rating of sexual arousal and the Sensory dimension of the subjective orgasmic experience (r = .41, p < .05), between rating of genital sensations and the Affective (r = .40, p < .05) and Sensory (r = .41, p < .05) dimensions, and between genital response and the Affective dimension (r = .42, p < .05). All these correlation coefficients are considered moderate (Cohen, 1988).

In the female sample, only a significant association was observed between the propensity for sexual excitation and the Sensory dimension of the subjective orgasmic experience (r = .36, p < .05). This correlation coefficient is considered low (Cohen, 1988) (Table 2).

Table 2.

Correlations between the subjective orgasm experience dimensions (Affective, Sensory, Intimacy, and Rewards) and different sexual arousal measures.

Variables 
1. Affective  –  .72⁎⁎⁎  .34*  .34*  .09  .05  −.25  .01 
2. Sensory  .57⁎⁎⁎  –  .27  .47⁎⁎  .36*  .18  −.08  .14 
3. Intimacy  .56⁎⁎  .45⁎⁎  –  .66⁎⁎⁎  .11  −0.01  −.11  −.08 
4. Rewards  .55⁎⁎  .35*  .31  –  .31  .21  .07  .14 
5. Propensity for sexual excitation  .09  .13  .10  .19  –  .25  .26  .15 
6. Rating of sexual arousal  .21  .41*  .20  .10  .09  –  .80⁎⁎⁎  .48⁎⁎ 
7. Rating of genital sensations  .40*  .41*  .09  .13  .22  .77⁎⁎⁎  –  .47⁎⁎ 
8. Genital response  .42*  .24  .23  .13  .09  .40*  .59⁎⁎⁎  – 

Note. Values below the diagonal are based on male scores. Values above the diagonal are based on female scores.

p < .05,

⁎⁎

p < .01,

⁎⁎⁎

p < .001.

Regression models

In the male sample, 15 % of the variance of the Affective dimension of the subjective orgasmic experience was significantly and positively explained by the genital response (F1,32 = 6.89, p < .05), and 14 % of the variance of the Sensory dimension was significantly and positively explained by the rating of sexual arousal (F1,32 = 6.52, p < .05) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Multiple regression models for subjective orgasm experience dimensions.

Males                 
Predictors  B  SE  β  95 % CI  t  p  R2  VIF 
Affective              .15   
Genital response  0.20  0.08  .42  0.05, 0.36  2.63  .013    1.00 
Sensory              .14   
Rating of sexual arousal  1.09  0.43  .41  0.22, 1.96  2.55  .016    1.00 
Females                 
Predictors  B  SE  β  95 % CI  t  p  R2  VIF 
Sensory              .11   
Propensity for sexual excitation  1.91  0.86  .36  0.15, 3.66  2.21  .034    1.00 

Note. B: non-standardized beta; SE: standard error; β: standardized beta; 95 % CI: 95 % confidence interval; R2: adjusted R-squared value; VIF: Variance inflation factor.

In the female sample, the propensity for sexual excitation (F1,32 = 4.88, p < .05) significantly explained, in a positive way, 11 % of the variance of the Sensory dimension (Table 3).

Discussion

The present study aimed to provide evidence for the validity of MMSOE in the context of same-sex relationships by examining the explanatory capacity of the propensity for sexual excitation, subjective sexual arousal, and genital response on the four dimensions of subjective orgasmic experience (i.e., Affective, Sensory, Intimacy, and Rewards). Overall, our findings underline the relationship found between different measures of sexual arousal and SOE, especially in the case of males, a fact congruent with the results of previous studies, both in the context of heterosexual relationships (Arcos-Romero et al., 2019) and masturbation (Cervilla et al., 2024), as well as with evidence suggesting that orgasmic experience differs according to sex (e.g., Arcos-Romero & Sierra, 2020; Cervilla et al., 2024; Mangas, Granados et al., 2022). In this sense, and in the context of sexual relationships, it had been already seen that woman, both heterosexual (Arcos-Romero & Sierra, 2020, 2023) and non-heterosexual (Mangas, Granados et al., 2022; Sierra, Muñoz-García & Mangas, 2024), experience orgasm in general terms more intensely. Despite this, when discussing in terms of frequency, the evidence suggesting the existence of a male-female orgasmic gap to the detriment of women is well known (Andrejek & Fetner, 2019; Döring & Mohseni, 2022; Wetzel & Sanchez, 2022).

First, the propensity for sexual excitation was only related to the Sensory dimension of SOE in females, explaining 11 % of its variance. This finding is inconsistent with previous evidence in heterosexual population concerning that propensity for sexual excitation is associated with subjective orgasmic experience in males, but not in females (Arcos-Romero et al., 2019; Cervilla et al., 2024; Moyano & Sierra, 2014). These results seem to support the relevance of the propensity for sexual excitation in females who have sexual relationships with other females, a fact also recently reported by Sierra, Mangas et al. (2024), by pointing out that bisexual/lesbian females present a greater propensity for sexual excitation than heterosexual females. The finding that propensity for sexual excitation explains the Sensory dimension is also congruent with the findings of Sierra, Muñoz-García and Mangas (2024), who reported that bisexual/lesbian females experience orgasm significantly more intensely than bisexual/gay males in sensory terms.

The lack of effect of this variable found in gay males may be due to the fact that current models of sexual functioning are not entirely accurate in them, whose sexual relationships take place in a different relational and sociocultural context than that of heterosexual males (Cohen et al., 2008; Cove & Boyle, 2002; Sierra, Mangas et al., 2023). It should also be noted that propensity for sexual excitation -as proposed within the Dual Control Model (Janssen & Bancroft, 2023) and as assessed by the SIS/SES-SF (Janssen et al., 2020)- has hardly been studied in people belonging to sexual minorities. The few studies that include people with sexual diversities have focused on relating propensity for sexual excitation with risk behaviors, such as substance use (Lorenz, 2021), cybersex addiction (Laier et al., 2015), or lack of sexual control (Miner et al., 2016), forgetting its relationship with the orgasmic experience. Our finding may also be due to the fact that, compared to heterosexual females, those who engage in sexual relationships with other females exhibit significantly more sexual desire (Lippa, 2007), score higher on sociosexuality (i.e., degree of openness to engage in casual or noncommittal sex; Simpson & Gangestad, 1991) and on sexual sensation seeking, sexual curiosity, and arousability (Stief et al., 2014). It is possible that the erotica of lesbian and bisexual females, unlike heterosexual females, is more focused on the goals and consequences of sexual activity (in this case, reaching orgasm) (Sierra, Muñoz-García & Mangas, 2024), a disposition that has traditionally been associated with male sexuality.

Regarding the rating of sexual arousal and the rating of genital sensations, only significant associations were found with the orgasmic experience in males. Specifically, the rating of sexual arousal correlated significantly with the Sensory dimension of orgasm, explaining 14 % of its variance. Although without explanatory capacity, significant correlations of the rating of genital sensations with the Affective and Sensory dimensions were also found. The prominence of these two dimensions of arousal only in the case of males is consistent with Mah and Binik's (2002) hypothesis that they are more focused on their own genital reactions. The lack of salience, in females, of situational sexual arousal (i.e., rating of sexual arousal and rating of genital sensations), as opposed to propensity for sexual excitation, differs from previous findings in heterosexual females by Arcos-Romero et al. (2019). This result should be further explored.

In non-heterosexual females, the weight of propensity for sexual excitation, as opposed to situational sexual arousal (i.e., subjective and objective sexual arousal recorded in the laboratory) may be due, on the one hand, to the fact that their socialization (see Adler et al., 2016; Clair, 2012) may have influenced the inhibition of their explicit sexual arousal, resulting in sexual arousal as a state playing no role. On the other hand, it could be due to factors associated with gender and/or sexual orientation directly related to the orgasmic experience, and may account for the fact that, although they have the predisposition to reach orgasm and are oriented to it (propensity for sexual excitation), it is not necessarily a detriment for them not to obtain it. This is congruent with the fact that females, regardless of their sexual orientation (Frederick et al., 2018), have lower expectations for orgasm compared to males (Wetzel et al., 2022). Moreover, they have less facility to obtain it (Andrejek & Fetner, 2019; Sánchez-Fuentes et al., 2019; Sierra et al., 2012), are characterized by subjectively experiencing it in a more complex way (Tavares et al., 2018), and prioritize other sexual activities beyond orgasm, such as kissing, cuddling and caressing (Garnets & Peplau, 2006), sometimes describing their best sexual encounters without reference to orgasm (Chatterji et al., 2017). In contrast, taking into account the social importance that has been given to male orgasm, sometimes considered as a need or right (Klein & Conley, 2022), coupled with the fact that males are much more encouraged to pursue their own sexual needs and desires (Miller & Byers, 2004; Sánchez-Fuentes & Santos-Iglesias, 2016), the association between sexual arousal as a state (i.e., appraisal of sexual arousal and genital sensations) and the orgasmic valuation made by them seems logical.

The genital response had a relevant role in the explanation of SOE only in males, correlating significantly with the Affective dimension of the orgasmic experience and explaining 15 % of its variance. This result is congruent with what was found in the context of heterosexual relationships, where also, only in males, the genital response was able to explain the Intimacy dimension (Arcos-Romero et al., 2019). In the present work, the genital response explained the Affective dimension, confirming again the importance of this orgasmic dimension in non-heterosexual males, something found in the few dyadic works that study SOE in same-sex couples (Mangas, Sierra & Granados, 2024; Pérez-Amorós et al., 2024).

In general, the dimensions of SOE that could be explained by sexual arousal were Affective and Sensory. Recently, it has been shown that the Affective dimension has the strongest link with sexual satisfaction in males with same-sex partners, and it has been observed that this dimension of orgasm has both an actor and partner effect on sexual satisfaction (Mangas, Sierra & Granados, 2024). In addition, a recent qualitative study has also revealed the notable prominence of the Affective dimension compared to the rest, something especially characteristic in the context of sexual relationships (Mangas, da Silva Alves et al., 2024), which argues in favor of the hypothesis that this dimension seems to mask the rest. For its part, the Sensory has also been shown to be a dimension of orgasm with outstanding dyadic salience in male couples (Mangas, Sierra & Granados, 2024), probably because it is the most physically evident orgasmic dimension (e.g., pulsating, flushing, trembling), which may cause males to focus on how their partners are physiologically experiencing their arousal and orgasm, and to take it as a marker of their own sexual satisfaction.

This study has some limitations that affect the generalizability of the results, since the sample consisted of young, healthy, with university studies and exclusively cisgender people. Additionally, we also consider it a limitation that we have not directly assessed or considered sexual orientation per se, only the sexual behavior of the participants, something that should be further explored in the future. Despite this, as Blair et al. (2017) point out, relationship configuration is a more influential factor than self-identified sexual orientation in explaining how sexual activity and orgasm vary. In addition, the artificiality of the laboratory studies prioritizes internal validity to the detriment of external validity. It should also be noted that the scarcity of similar studies focused on the experiences of non-heterosexual people has meant that our findings have been equated, not always desirably, with what has been established in studies with heterosexual populations. Future research should include older people, people with sexual dysfunctions, as well as people with other identities that account for sexual diversity. Since the MMSOE allows the study of orgasmic experiences also in the context of masturbation, a good way to continue this line of research would be to validate this model in the context of solitary masturbation of non-heterosexual people. We also propose the possibility of conducting studies using scenarios, both neutral and erotic, presented through virtual reality.

Conclusions

The results obtained are considered relevant from a clinical and research point of view, as they provide evidence of validity to the MMSOE in the context of same-sex relationships, confirming the utility of this model. In addition, we observed differential nuances compared to previous evidence in the context of heterosexual relationships provided by Arcos-Romero et al. (2019). Specifically, we found a relationship between the propensity for sexual excitation and the Sensory dimension of orgasm in females. In males, in contrast, and in line with previous evidence indicating that orgasm dimensions are explained by more measures of sexual arousal, we observed a greater number of sexual arousal-orgasm associations, with situational sexual arousal (i.e., rating of sexual arousal and rating of genital sensations) and genital response (i.e., penile circumference) having more relevance. Therefore, when it comes to explaining the subjective orgasmic experience, in females who have sexual relationships with other females, the propensity for sexual excitation would have a greater influence, while in males who have sex with other males, situational sexual arousal and genital response would have a greater impact. It is important to note that the results of this study should be interpreted with caution due to the absence of high correlation coefficients. Finally, at the clinical level, and because the LGBTIQA+ Affirmative Psychotherapy approach is becoming increasingly recognized, which leads the use of scientific knowledge about these minorities in psychological practice (Moradi & Budge, 2018; Pepping et al., 2018), our findings could provide health professionals with a solid theoretical model adapted to the population that relates sexually with people of the same sex. Therefore, this study makes a contribution to research that includes sexual minorities that fall outside the traditional heterosexual schema (Pollitt et al., 2023), this being even more relevant in the field of sexual health, where most works do not include these minority groups, particularly if they involve females who have sexual relationships with other females (Obón-Azuara et al., 2023).

Funding

This study was made possible by the Grant FPU19/00369 funded by MICIU/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and by ESF Investing in Your Future. It is part of the first author's thesis (Psychological Doctoral Programme B13 56 1; RD 99/2011).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the participants who took part in this experimental study. They would also like to thank the collaborating companies (Amantis, Fleshlight, Lelo, Platanomelón, and Vive Sex Shop) for donating prevention material to distribute to the participants who attended the experiment.

References
[Adler et al., 2016]
N.E. Adler, M.M. Glymour, J. Fielding.
Addressing social determinants of health and health inequalities.
JAMA, 316 (2016), pp. 1641-1642
[Álvarez-Muelas et al., 2022]
A. Álvarez-Muelas, J.C. Sierra, C. Gómez-Berrocal, A.I. Arcos-Romero, C. Calvillo, R. Granados.
Study of sexual concordance in men and women with different typologies of adherence to the sexual double standard.
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 22 (2022),
[Andrejek and Fetner, 2019]
N. Andrejek, T. Fetner.
The gender gap in orgasms: Survey data from a mid-sized Canadian city.
International Journal of Sexual Health, 31 (2019), pp. 26-35
[Arcos-Romero et al., 2019]
A.I. Arcos-Romero, R. Granados, J.C. Sierra.
Relationship between orgasm experience and sexual excitation: Validation of the model of the subjective orgasm experience.
International Journal of Impotence Research, 31 (2019), pp. 282-287
[Arcos-Romero et al., 2018]
A.I. Arcos-Romero, N. Moyano, J.C. Sierra.
Psychometric properties of the Orgasm Rating Scale in context of sexual relationship in a Spanish sample.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 15 (2018), pp. 741-749
[Arcos-Romero and Sierra, 2018]
A.I. Arcos-Romero, J.C. Sierra.
Systematic review of the subjective experience of orgasm.
Revista Internacional de Andrología, 16 (2018), pp. 75-81
[Arcos-Romero and Sierra, 2020]
A.I. Arcos-Romero, J.C. Sierra.
Factors associated with subjective orgasm experience in heterosexual relationships.
Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 46 (2020), pp. 314-329
[Arcos-Romero and Sierra, 2023]
A.I. Arcos-Romero, J.C. Sierra.
How do heterosexual men and women rate their orgasm in a relational context?.
International Journal of Impotence Research, 35 (2023), pp. 164-169
[Bancroft and Janssen, 2000]
J. Bancroft, E. Janssen.
The dual control model of male sexual response: A theoretical approach to centrally mediated erectile dysfunction.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24 (2000), pp. 571-579
[Bancroft et al., 2003]
J. Bancroft, J. Loftus, J.S. Long.
Distress about sex: A national survey of women in heterosexual relationships.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32 (2003), pp. 193-208
[Blair et al., 2017]
K.L. Blair, J. Cappell, C.F. Pukall.
Not all orgasms were created equal: Differences in frequency and satisfaction of orgasm experiences by sexual activity in same-sex versus mixedsex relationships.
The Journal of Sex Research, 55 (2017), pp. 719-733
[Brody et al., 2013]
S. Brody, K. Klapilova, L. Krejčová.
More frequent vaginal orgasm is associated with experiencing greater excitement from deep vaginal stimulation.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 10 (2013), pp. 1730-1736
[Carpenter et al., 2011]
D. Carpenter, E. Janssen, C. Graham, H. Vorst, J. Wicherts.
The Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales-Short Form (SIS/SES-SF).
Handbook of sexuality-related measures, 3rd ed., pp. 236-239
[Cervilla et al., 2023]
O. Cervilla, E. Jiménez-Antón, A. Álvarez-Muelas, P. Mangas, R. Granados, J.C. Sierra.
Solitary sexual desire: Its relation to subjective orgasm experience and sexual arousal in the masturbation context within a Spanish population.
Healthcare, 11 (2023), pp. 1805
[Cervilla et al., 2024]
O. Cervilla, J.C. Sierra, A. Álvarez-Muelas, P. Mangas, G.M. Sánchez-Pérez, R. Granados.
Validation of the multidimensional model of the subjective orgasm experience in the context of masturbation.
Revista Iberoamericana de Psicología y Salud, 15 (2024), pp. 18-26
[Chatterji et al., 2017]
S. Chatterji, L.Y. Bay-Cheng, V. Schick, B. Dodge, A. Baldwin, B. Van Der Pol, J.D. Fortenberry.
The year's best: Interpersonal elements of bisexual women's most satisfying sexual experiences in the past year.
The Journal of Sex Research, 54 (2017), pp. 887-898
[Clair, 2012]
I. Clair.
Sociologie du genre: Sociologies contemporaines.
Armand Colin, (2012),
[Cohen, 1988]
J. Cohen.
Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
2nd ed., Lawrence Erlbaum, (1988),
[Cohen et al., 2008]
J.N. Cohen, E.S. Byers, L.P. Walsh.
Factors influencing the sexual relationships of lesbians and gay men.
International Journal of Sexual Health, 20 (2008), pp. 162-176
[Cove and Boyle, 2002]
J. Cove, M. Boyle.
Gay men's self-defined sexual problems, perceived causes and factors in remission.
Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 17 (2002), pp. 137-147
[Döring and Mohseni, 2022]
N. Döring, M.R. Mohseni.
The gender orgasm gap: A critical research review on gender differences in orgasm frequency during heterosex.
Zeitschrift fur Sexualforschung, 35 (2022), pp. 73-87
[Faul et al., 2007]
F. Faul, E. Erdfelder, A.G. Lang, A. Buchner.
G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.
Behavior Research Methods, 39 (2007), pp. 175-191
[Frederick et al., 2018]
D.A. Frederick, H.K.S. John, J.R. Garcia, E.A. Lloyd.
Differences in orgasm frequency among gay, lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual men and women in a US national sample.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47 (2018), pp. 273-288
[Frith, 2015]
H. Frith.
Orgasmic bodies: The orgasm in contemporary western culture.
Springer, (2015),
[Garnets and Peplau, 2006]
L. Garnets, L.A. Peplau.
Sexuality in the lives of aging lesbian and bisexual women.
Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender aging: Research and clinical perspectives, pp. 70-90 http://dx.doi.org/10.7312/kimm13618-005
[Granados et al., 2020]
R. Granados, J. Carvalho, J.C. Sierra.
Preliminary evidence on how the Dual Control Model predicts female sexual response to a bogus negative feedback.
Psychological Reports, 124 (2020), pp. 502-520
[Janssen, 2011]
E. Janssen.
Sexual arousal in men: A review and conceptual analysis.
Hormones and Behavior, 59 (2011), pp. 708-716
[Janssen and Bancroft, 2023]
E. Janssen, J. Bancroft.
The Dual Control Model of sexual response: A scoping review, 2009–2022.
The Journal of Sex Research, 60 (2023), pp. 948-968
[Janssen et al., 2020]
E. Janssen, D. Carpenter, C.A. Graham, H. Vorst, J. Wicherts.
The Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales-Short Form.
Handbook of sexuality-related measures, 4th ed., pp. 77-80
[Janssen and Prause, 2016]
E. Janssen, N. Prause.
Sexual response.
Handbook of psychophysiology, 4th ed., pp. 284-299
[Klein and Conley, 2022]
V. Klein, T.D. Conley.
The role of gendered entitlement in understanding inequality in the bedroom.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13 (2022), pp. 1047-1057
[Kontula and Miettinen, 2016]
O. Kontula, A. Miettinen.
Determinants of female sexual orgasms.
Socioafective Neuroscience & Psychology, 6 (2016),
[Korff and Geer, 1983]
J. Korff, J.H. Geer.
The relationship between sexual arousal experience and genital response.
Psychophysiology, 20 (1983), pp. 121-127
[Kuban et al., 1999]
M. Kuban, H.E. Barbaree, R. Blanchard.
A comparison of volume and circumference phallometry: Response magnitude and method agreement.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 28 (1999), pp. 345-359
[Laier et al., 2015]
C. Laier, J. Pekal, M. Brand.
Sexual excitability and dysfunctional coping determine cybersex addiction in homosexual males.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18 (2015), pp. 575-580
[Lippa, 2007]
R. Lippa.
The relation between sex drive and sexual attraction to men and women: A cross-national study of heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual men and women.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36 (2007), pp. 209-222
[Lorentzen, 2007]
J. Lorentzen.
Masculinities and the phenomenology of men's orgasms.
Men and Masculinities, 10 (2007), pp. 71-84
[Lorenz, 2021]
T.K. Lorenz.
Sexual excitation and sex-linked substance use predict overall cannabis use in mostly heterosexual and bisexual women.
The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 47 (2021), pp. 433-443
[Mah and Binik, 2001]
K. Mah, Y.M. Binik.
The nature of human orgasm: A critical review of major trends.
Clinical Psychology Review, 21 (2001), pp. 823-856
[Mah and Binik, 2002]
K. Mah, Y.M. Binik.
Do all orgasms feel alike? Evaluating a two-dimensional model of the orgasm experience across gender and sexual context.
The Journal of Sex Research, 39 (2002), pp. 104-113
[Mah and Binik, 2020]
K. Mah, Y.M. Binik.
The Orgasm Rating Scale.
Handbook of sexuality-related measures, 4th ed., pp. 503-507
[Mangas et al., 2022]
P. Mangas, O. Cervilla, A. Álvarez-Muelas, L.E. Muñoz-García, R. Granados, A.I. Arcos-Romero, J.C. Sierra.
Films to validate the multidimensional model of the subjective orgasm experience in the context of homosexual relationships. [Oral communication].
X Jornadas Internacionales de Investigación en Sexología, (2022),
[Mangas et al., 2024]
P. Mangas, M.E. da Silva Alves, L. Fernandes de Araújo, J.C. Sierra.
The empire of affectivity: Qualitative evidence of the subjective orgasm experience.
Behavioral Sciences, 14 (2024),
[Mangas et al., 2022b]
P. Mangas, R. Granados, O. Cervilla, J.C. Sierra.
Validation of the Orgasm Rating Scale in context of sexual relationships of gay and lesbian adults.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19 (2022),
[Mangas and Sierra, 2021]
P. Mangas, J.C. Sierra.
Validation of clips in the context of same-sex relationships for objective sexual arousal of gay and lesbian youths. [Oral communication].
XV Congreso Español y Euroamericano de Sexología,
[Mangas et al., 2024]
P. Mangas, J.C. Sierra, R. Granados.
Effects of subjective orgasm experience in sexual satisfaction: A dyadic analysis in same-sex Hispanic couples.
Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 50 (2024), pp. 346-368
[Meston et al., 2004]
C.M. Meston, E. Hull, R.J. Levin, M. Sipski.
Disorders of orgasm in women.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 1 (2004), pp. 66-68
[Miller and Byers, 2004]
S.A. Miller, E.S. Byers.
Actual and desired duration of foreplay and intercourse: Discordance and misperceptions within heterosexual couples.
The Journal of Sex Research, 41 (2004), pp. 301-309
[Miner et al., 2016]
M.H. Miner, R.S. Romine, N. Raymond, E. Janssen, A. MacDonald III, E Coleman.
Understanding the personality and behavioral mechanisms defining hypersexuality in men who have sex with men.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 13 (2016), pp. 1323-1331
[Moradi and Budge, 2018]
B. Moradi, S.L. Budge.
Engaging in LGBQ+ affirmative psychotherapies with all clients: Defining themes and practices.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74 (2018), pp. 2028-2042
[Mosher, 2011]
D.L. Mosher.
Multiple Indicators of Subjective Sexual Arousal.
Handbook of sexuality-related measures, 3rd ed., pp. 59-61
[Moyano and Sierra, 2014]
N. Moyano, J.C. Sierra.
Validation of the Sexual Inhibition/Sexual Excitation Scales-Short Form (SIS/SES-SF).
Terapia Psicológica, 32 (2014), pp. 87-100
[Muñoz-García et al., 2023]
L.E. Muñoz-García, C. Gómez-Berrocal, J.C. Sierra.
Evaluating the subjective orgasm experience through sexual context, gender, and sexual orientation.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 52 (2023), pp. 1479-1491
[Obón-Azuara et al., 2023]
B. Obón-Azuara, C. Vergara-Maldonado, I. Gutiérrez-Cía, I. Iguacel, Á. Gasch-Gallén.
Gaps in sexual health research about women who have sex with women. A scoping review.
Gaceta Sanitaria, 36 (2023), pp. 439-445
[Paterson et al., 2014]
L.Q. Paterson, E.S. Jin, R. Amsel, Y.M. Binik.
Gender similarities and differences in sexual arousal, desire, and orgasmic pleasure in the laboratory.
The Journal of Sex Research, 51 (2014), pp. 801-813
[Pepping et al., 2018]
C.A. Pepping, A. Lyons, E.M. Morris.
Affirmative LGBT psychotherapy: Outcomes of a therapist training protocol.
Psychotherapy, 55 (2018), pp. 52-62
[Pérez-Amorós et al., 2024]
C. Pérez-Amorós, J.C. Sierra, P. Mangas.
Subjective orgasm experience in different-sex and same-sex couples: A dyadic approach.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, (2024),
[Pollitt et al., 2023]
A.M. Pollitt, K.L. Blair, P.J. Lannutti.
A review of two decades of LGBTQ-inclusive research in JSPR and PR.
Personal Relationships, 30 (2023), pp. 144-173
[Prause and Janssen, 2005]
N. Prause, E. Janssen.
Blood flow: Vaginal photoplethysmography.
Women's sexual function and dysfunction: Study, diagnosis and treatment, pp. 361-369
[Sánchez-Fuentes et al., 2019]
M.M. Sánchez-Fuentes, N. Moyano, R. Granados, J.C. Sierra.
Validation of the Spanish version of the Arizona Sexual Experience Scale (ASEX) using self-reported and psychophysiological measures.
Revista Iberoamericana de Psicología y Salud, 10 (2019), pp. 1-14
[Sánchez-Fuentes and Santos-Iglesias, 2016]
M.M. Sánchez-Fuentes, P. Santos-Iglesias.
Sexual satisfaction in Spanish heterosexual couples: Testing the interpersonal exchange model of sexual satisfaction.
Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 42 (2016), pp. 223-242
[Schiavi and Segraves, 1995]
R.C. Schiavi, R.T. Segraves.
The biology of sexual function.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 18 (1995), pp. 7-23
[Sierra et al., 2019]
J.C. Sierra, A. Álvarez-Muelas, A.I. Arcos-Romero, C. Calvillo, R. Torres-Obregón, R. Granados.
Connection between subjective sexual arousal and genital response: Differences between men and women.
Revista Internacional de Andrología, 17 (2019), pp. 24-30
[Sierra et al., 2023]
J.C. Sierra, A.I. Arcos-Romero, R. Granados, O. Cervilla, P. Mangas, L.E. Muñoz-García, A. Álvarez-Muelas.
Validation of the Spanish version of the Massachusetts General Hospital-Sexual Functioning Questionnaire (MGH-SFQ) with sexual arousal measures in men.
International Journal of Psychological Research, 16 (2023), pp. 42-50
[Sierra et al., 2017]
J.C. Sierra, A.I. Arcos-Romero, R. Granados, M.M. Sánchez-Fuentes, C. Calvillo, N. Moyano.
Ratings of Sexual Arousal and Ratings of Genital Sensations: Psychometric properties in Spanish sample.
Revista Internacional de Andrología, 15 (2017), pp. 99-107
[Sierra et al., 2024]
J.C. Sierra, O. Cervilla, A. Álvarez-Muelas, M.M. Sánchez-Fuentes.
Validity and reliability evidence and norms of the Spanish version of the sexual inhibition/sexual excitation scales-short form.
Psicothema, 36 (2024), pp. 154-164
[Sierra et al., 2023]
J.C. Sierra, P. Mangas, A. Guillén-Riquelme, L.E. Muñoz-García.
Measurement invariance of the Arizona Sexual Experience Scale by sexual orientation: Comparing the sexual functioning of gay and heterosexual individuals.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 20 (2023), pp. 684-689
[Sierra et al., 2024b]
Sierra, J.C., Mangas, P., Muñoz-García, L.E., & Cervilla, O. (2024). The Spanish version of the sexual inhibition/sexual excitation scales-short form: Establishing measurement invariance across sexual orientation. Manuscript Submitted for Publication.
[Sierra et al., 2024c]
J.C. Sierra, L.E. Muñoz-García, P. Mangas.
And how do LGB adults rate their orgasms in a relational context?.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 21 (2024), pp. 255-261
[Sierra et al., 2012]
J.C. Sierra, P. Vallejo-Medina, P. Santos-Iglesias, M. Lameiras Fernández.
Validation of Massachusetts General Hospital-Functioning Questionnaire (MGH-SFQ) in a Spanish population.
Atención Primaria, 44 (2012), pp. 516-526
[Simons and Carey, 2001]
J.S. Simons, M.P. Carey.
Prevalence of sexual dysfunctions: Results from a decade of research.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 30 (2001), pp. 177-219
[Simpson and Gangestad, 1991]
J.A. Simpson, S.W. Gangestad.
Individual differences in sociosexuality: Evidence for convergent and discriminant validity.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 (1991), pp. 870-883
[Stief et al., 2014]
M.C. Stief, G. Rieger, R.C. Savin-Williams.
Bisexuality is associated with elevated sexual sensation seeking, sexual curiosity, and sexual excitability.
Personality and Individual Differences, 66 (2014), pp. 193-198
[Tavares et al., 2018]
I.M. Tavares, E.T. Laan, P.J. Nobre.
Sexual inhibition is a vulnerability factor for orgasm problems in women.
The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 15 (2018), pp. 361-372
[Wade et al., 2005]
L.D. Wade, E.C. Kremer, J. Brown.
The incidental orgasm: The presence of clitoral knowledge and the absence of orgasm for women.
Women & Health, 42 (2005), pp. 117-138
[Wetzel et al., 2022]
G.M. Wetzel, R.A. Cultice, D.T. Sanchez.
Orgasm frequency predicts desire and expectation for orgasm: Assessing the orgasm gap within mixed-sex couples.
Sex Roles, 86 (2022), pp. 456-470
[Wetzel and Sanchez, 2022]
G.M. Wetzel, D.T. Sanchez.
Heterosexual young adults’ experience with and perceptions of the orgasm gap: A mixed methods approach.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 46 (2022), pp. 131-146
Copyright © 2024. The Authors
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo