covid
Buscar en
Mindfulness & Compassion
Toda la web
Inicio Mindfulness & Compassion Set shifting reaction-time improves following meditation or simple breathcountin...
Información de la revista
Vol. 2. Núm. 2.
Páginas 82-85 (julio - diciembre 2017)
Compartir
Compartir
Descargar PDF
Más opciones de artículo
Visitas
2599
Vol. 2. Núm. 2.
Páginas 82-85 (julio - diciembre 2017)
Review
Acceso a texto completo
Set shifting reaction-time improves following meditation or simple breathcounting in meditators and meditation-naïve participants: Data from naturalistic, ecological momentary-assessment devices
El tiempo de reacción al cambio de set cognitivo mejora después de la meditación o tras contar respiraciones sencillamente en meditadores y neófitos en la meditación: datos de mecanismos de evaluación ecológica instantánea naturalista
Visitas
2599
Rachel Atchley
Autor para correspondencia
rachel.atchley@gmail.com

Corresponding author.
, Daniel Klee, Barry Oken
Department of Neurology, Oregon Health & Science University, United States
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Figuras (3)
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Tablas (1)
Table 1. Participant characteristics.
Abstract

A comparison is made between the performance of meditators and controls in a letter-number task-switching test. Data were recorded over a five-day period using a previously developed ecological momentary assessment paradigm. Participants consisted of naïve, novice, and experienced meditators, who completed a task-switching reaction time (RT) task before and after 20-min breath-counting sessions. There was a decrease in reaction time over testing days, p<.007, as well as a separate decrease in reaction time pre- to post-meditation, p<.001. RTs decreased each day, as expected, and post-meditation/breath-counting RTs were consistently faster than pre-meditation/breath-counting RTs. These results suggest a meditation effect separate from a learning effect.

Keywords:
Meditation
Reaction time
Naturalistic data
Resumen

Comparamos el rendimiento de meditadores y controles en una prueba de conmutación de tareas de letras y números. Se registraron los datos de un período de 5 días utilizando un paradigma de evaluación ecológica instantánea desarrollado anteriormente. Los participantes eran neófitos, principiantes y experimentados en la meditación, y realizaron una tarea de tiempo de reacción ante la conmutación de tareas antes y después de sesiones de contar respiraciones de 20min. Hubo una disminución en el tiempo de reacción durante los días de la prueba, p<0,007, así como una disminución independiente en el tiempo de reacción antes y después de la meditación, p<0,001. Los tiempos de reacción fueron disminuyendo cada día, como se esperaba, y los tiempos de reacción posteriores a la meditación/contar respiraciones fueron consecuentemente más rápidos que los tiempos de reacción anteriores a la meditación/contar respiraciones. Estos resultados sugieren un efecto de la meditación independiente del efecto del aprendizaje.

Palabras clave:
Meditación
Tiempo de reacción
Datos naturalistas
Texto completo
Introduction

Studies on meditation and attention have found that experienced meditators have greater attentional control than inexperienced meditators (Brefczynski-Lewis, Lutz, Schaefer, Levinson, & Davidson, 2007; Chan & Woollacott, 2007; Chiesa, Calati, & Serretti, 2011; Lutz, Slatger, Dunne, & Davidson, 2008). Moreover, attentional control has been shown to differ in meditators and people who do not meditate (Cahn & Polich, 2009; Delgado-Pastor, Perakakis, Subramanya, Telles, & Vila, 2013; Tang et al., 2009).

To further inform this line of research, a logical step is naturalistic data collection and assessment of cognitive changes in meditation interventions. To begin to address this need, we compared performance of meditators and controls on a letter-number task-switching test before and after at-home meditation. Data were recorded over five days using a previously developed ecological momentary assessment (EMA) paradigm (Oken, Miller, Goodrich, & Wahbeh, 2014; Wahbeh, Zwickey, & Oken, 2011). We hypothesized that set-shifting reaction times (RTs) would decrease across days due to a learning effect; RTs were also expected to decrease after each meditation session due to increased attentional focus. In a sample of non-meditators and meditators, we predicted a greater reduction of reaction time in meditators following the meditation sessions compared to the reduction of reaction time in controls following simple untrained breath counting sessions.

MethodsRT study design

This cross-sectional study evaluated differences across three meditation groups: 1) naïve (no experience within 2 years); 2) novice (formal training and a minimum of 1000h of practice); 3) experienced (≥5000h of practice). Participants were recruited from the Portland, OR, USA metro area and meditator groups were pooled in these analyses using the same rationale as Atchley et al. (2016).

Participants were screened and consented when they came in for a single laboratory visit for EEG and physiological assessments, then took home pre-programmed handheld devices that sounded an alert once a day to signal participants to begin a cognitive assessment before and then after a 20-min experimental condition. The experimental conditions were breath counting and meditation for the controls and meditators, respectively. Meditators potentially engaged in breath-counting if they chose that practice. The ecological momentary assessment (EMA) software was implemented on a repurposed smartphone and required no cellular or internet connectivity. Individual testing times were chosen in advance by the participants. The devices sounded a signal to begin the EMA session. Participants could opt to delay the session by 30min up to three times. At the end of five days, the device was mailed back to the laboratory by the participant, or the research assistants would arrange to pick up the device from the participant's home. Devices recorded all participant inputs, times, completion statuses, and assessment data.

RT task procedure

The task-switching test, modeled after Rogers and Monsell (1995), took 10min to complete and contained 160 trials (see Fig. 1 for visual). Consecutive congruent trials occurred randomly 1–4 times followed by a task-switching trial. The task-switching test was completed before and after either a 20-min meditation session (meditation group) or a 20-min breath counting session (control group). Median RTs for the correct task-switching trials were calculated for each of the two sessions on all 5 days.

Figure 1.

Task-switching test.

(0.1MB).
Statistical analysis

RTs were analyzed using a hierarchical mixed ANOVA comparing day (1–5, within subjects), meditation/breath-counting (pre- or post-, within subjects), and group (meditator or control, between subjects). All analyses were conducted in SPSS Version 22 (IBM).

ResultsParticipants

We recruited 42 participants (M age=49, 62% female) who met the following criteria: (1) aged between 25 and 75 years; (2) good past and present medical health; (3) stable on all meditations for at least 2 months; (4) cognitively intact, as determined by a score of at least 31 on the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS) (Welsh, Breitner, & Magruder-Habib, 1993). Exclusion criteria were: (1) significant medical or neurological disorder/disease; (2) significant visual or hearing impairment; (3) medications that would alter outcomes such as benzodiazepines or neuroleptics; and (4) significant untreated depression. See Table 1 for more details on the participant sample. Participants in the control group had no experience with any form of meditation, while participants in the meditation groups had formal training, practiced at least 3 times a week, and had a lifetime practice minimum of either 1000h (novices) or 5000h (experienced) using the same rationale reported in Atchley et al. (2016). Acceptable meditation practices for the study's purpose included any styles with an attentional focus, including mind-body practices such as yoga.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics.

  All participants (SD)  Non-meditators (SD)  Novice meditators (SD)  Experienced meditators (SD) 
N  42  13  15  14 
Age (M49 (13)  48 (11)  50 (13)  49 (15) 
Female  62%  69%  73%  43% 
Minority  24%  23%  27%  21% 
Practice in Years (M–  –  2.4 years (2.5)  22.6 years (13.2) 

There was a decrease in task-switching trial reaction time across days, p<.001 (Fig. 2). There was a separate decrease in reaction time pre- to post-meditation or breath-counting for controls, p<.001, but not meditators (Fig. 3). Interestingly, there were no group effects on task-switching RTs, although there was a day by pre-/post-breath counting or meditation interaction, p=.012 (Fig. 3).

Figure 2.

Task-switching reaction times.

(0.15MB).
Figure 3.

Task-switching reaction time group comparisons.

(0.18MB).
Discussion

Most of our hypotheses were confirmed. The results indicate improved ability to switch attentional set following either the meditation or breath counting experimental condition. The interaction suggests that task switching RTs decreased each day; however, post-meditation RTs were consistently faster than pre-meditation RTs, which suggests a meditation effect separate from a learning effect. However, there was no difference in improvements between the meditation-naïve participants and the meditators. This post-experimental condition (meditation or breath counting) change is in addition to the learning effect in RT seen over the five days. The fact that simple breath counting in mediation-naïve people improved RT shows that it is critical to pay careful attention to the design of control conditions for mechanistic studies of meditation on cognition. Since some degree of same-day learning may even be occurring pre- to post-experimental condition, it may well be necessary to utilize two control conditions.

Conclusions

RTs decreased each day, as expected, and post-meditation/breath-counting RTs were consistently faster than pre-meditation/breath-counting RTs. This effect was more pronounced early on and present in both controls and meditators.

While highly interesting in terms of changing cognition in relation to meditative practice, these findings are still preliminary. These results draw strength from the naturalistic setting for data collection in addition to in-laboratory assessments, but replication studies are needed.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Funding

This study was funded by NIH-NCCIH T32 AT002688 and K24 AT005121.

Acknowledgements

We wish to acknowledge the work of Roger Ellingson in the development of our device software.

References
[Atchley et al., 2016]
R. Atchley, D. Klee, T. Memmott, E. Goodrich, H. Wahbeh, B. Oken.
Event-related potential correlates of mindfulness meditation competence.
[Brefczynski-Lewis et al., 2007]
J.A. Brefczynski-Lewis, A. Lutz, H.S. Schaefer, D.B. Levinson, R.J. Davidson.
Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 104 (2007), pp. 11483-11488
[Cahn and Polich, 2009]
B.R. Cahn, J. Polich.
Meditation (Vipassana) and the P3a event-related brain potential.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72 (2009), pp. 51-60
[Chan and Woollacott, 2007]
D. Chan, M. Woollacott.
Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: Is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved?.
Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13 (2007), pp. 651-657
[Chiesa et al., 2011]
A. Chiesa, R. Calati, A. Serretti.
Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities?. A systematic review of neuropsychological findings.
Clinical Psychology Review, 32 (2011), pp. 449-464
[Delgado-Pastor et al., 2013]
L.C. Delgado-Pastor, P. Perakakis, P. Subramanya, S. Telles, J. Vila.
Mindfulness (Vipassana) meditation: Effects on P3b event-related potential and heart rate variability.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90 (2013), pp. 207-214
[Lutz et al., 2008]
A. Lutz, H.A. Slatger, J.D. Dunne, R.J. Davidson.
Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 (2008), pp. 163-169
[Oken et al., 2014]
B. Oken, M. Miller, E. Goodrich, H. Wahbeh.
Effects of mindfulness meditation on self-rated stress-related measures: Improvements in neuroticism and ecological momentary assessment of stress.
Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 20 (2014), pp. A64
[Rogers and Monsell, 1995]
R.D. Rogers, S. Monsell.
The costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125 (1995), pp. 207-231
[Tang et al., 2009]
Y.Y. Tang, Y. Ma, Y. Fan, H. Feng, J. Wang, S. Feng, et al.
Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106 (2009), pp. 8865-8870
[Wahbeh et al., 2011]
H. Wahbeh, H. Zwickey, B. Oken.
One method of objective adherence measurement in mind-body medicine.
The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 17 (2011), pp. 175-177
[Welsh et al., 1993]
K.A. Welsh, J.C. Breitner, K.M. Magruder-Habib.
Detection of dementia in the elderly using telephone screening of cognitive status.
Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 6 (1993), pp. 103-110
Copyright © 2017. Mindfulness & Compassion
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo