covid
Buscar en
The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context
Toda la web
Inicio The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context Generalists versus specialists: Toward a typology of batterers in prison
Información de la revista
Vol. 8. Núm. 1.
Páginas 19-26 (enero 2015)
Compartir
Compartir
Descargar PDF
Más opciones de artículo
Visitas
6432
Vol. 8. Núm. 1.
Páginas 19-26 (enero 2015)
Open Access
Generalists versus specialists: Toward a typology of batterers in prison
Generalistas frente a especialistas: hacia una tipología de maltratadores encarcelados
Visitas
6432
Juan Herrero
Autor para correspondencia
olaizola@uniovi.es

Corresponding author. Despacho 233. Plaza Feijoo s/n. 33003 Oviedo, Spain.
, Andrea Torres, Asunción Fernández-Suárez, Francisco J. Rodríguez-Díaz
University of Oviedo, Spain
Este artículo ha recibido

Under a Creative Commons license
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Tablas (4)
Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and F Test on Individual Variables.
Table 2. Means, Standard Deviations, and F Test on Family Variables.
Table 3. Means, Standard Deviations, and F Test on Community Variables.
Table 4. MANOVAs of Individual, Family, and Community Variables.
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Abstract

In this study we apply the versatile/specialist offender debate to the research of intimate partner violence. We propose the existence of two types of imprisoned male batterers: the generalist and the specialist batterer. The individual, family, and community characteristics of these types of batterers are further explored in 110 imprisoned males in the Penitentiary of Villabona (Spain). As for the individual characteristics, results indicate that the generalist batterer present higher levels of psychopathology (specially antisocial and borderline personality), sexist attitudes, and substance dependence. Specialist batterers presented higher levels of conflict in their family of origin. Finally, generalist batterers reported coming from more socially disordered communities and showed lower levels of participation and integration in these communities than the specialist batterer. These results suggest that the classical distinctions among batterers based on psychopathology and context of violence (whether general or family only) might be of little utility when applied to imprisoned male batterers.

Keywords:
Batterer
Imprisoned
Typology
Psychopathology
Community
Family
Criminal history
Resumen

En este estudio aplicamos el debate del delincuente versátil/especialista a la investigación de la violencia de pareja. Proponemos que hay dos tipos de maltratadores masculinos en prisión: el generalista y el especialista. Se profundiza en la exploración de las características familiares, individuales y comunitarias de ambos tipos de maltratadores en 110 varones encarcelados en la prisión de Villabona (España). Sobre las características individuales los resultados indican que el maltratador generalista tiene niveles elevados de psicopatología (sobre todo personalidad antisocial y límite), actitudes sexistas y dependencia de sustancias. Los maltratadores especialistas tenían niveles elevados de conflicto con la familia de origen. Por último, los maltratadores generalistas afirmaban que procedían de comunidades más desestructuradas socialmente que los maltratadores especialistas. Estos resultados indican que la distinción clásica entre maltratadores según la psicopatología y el contexto de la violencia (únicamente la general o familiar) pudiera ser poco útil en el caso de los maltratadores masculinos encarcelados.

Palabras clave:
Maltratador
Encarcelado
Tipología
Psicopatología
Comunidad
Familia
Historia delictiva
Texto completo

In the last decades, the study of typologies of batterers has provided empirical evidence on the heterogeneous nature of partner violence and has pointed out how typologies could be of help in identifying different etiological mechanisms of partner violence (Capaldi & Kim, 2007). In their influential review, Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart (1994) proposed that batterers might be classified along three dimensions: (a) severity and frequency of marital violence, (b) generality of the violence (i.e., family-only or extrafamilial violence), and (c) batterer's psychopathology or personality disorders. Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart suggested that using these dimensions would produce three batterer subtypes: (a) family only, (b) dysphoric–borderline, and (c) generally violent–antisocial men. They estimated that around 50% of violent male partners recruited in a community sample would fall into the family-only batterer category, 25% into the dysphoric/borderline category, and 25% into the generally violent/antisocial category.

More recently, Cavanaugh and Gelles (2005) (see also Bender & Roberts, 2007; Cunha & Gonçalves, 2013) used three similar dimensions – severity and frequency of violence, criminal history, and level of psychopathology – to propose three types of batterers: low-, moderate-, and high-risk offenders. Low-risk offenders showed low severity, low frequency, little or no psychopathology, and usually no criminal history. Moderate-risk offenders exhibited moderate levels of severity and frequency of violence as well as moderate to high psychopathology. High-risk offenders revealed high severity and frequency of violence, high levels of psychopathy as well as a criminal history. General ideas behind these classifications are that: a) the more general the violence (i.e., existence of criminal history), the more likely partner violence be moderate to severe; and, b) the presence of moderate to severe partner violence is related to moderate to high psychopathology.

Severity of Violence and Criminal History

Batterer's criminal history has been traditionally linked to the existence of severe violence toward partner (Bender & Roberts, 2007; Cavanaugh & Gelles, 2005; Holtzworth-Munroe & Stuart, 1994). The most severe type of violence in Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's (1994) typology belongs to the generally violent/antisocial batterer with a long criminal history, a profile that is also found by other researchers. In Gondolf's (1988) typology, Type I or sociopathic batterer is also violent outside the home and presents a longer criminal history than the antisocial batterer (Type II) and the typical batterer (Type III), who is similar to the family-only batterer in Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's typology. Conversely, in Hamberger, Lohr, Bonge, and Tonlin's (1996) study of 204 maritally violent men, when violence is directed exclusively toward their partners it used to be less frequent, less severe, and with no psychopathology associated (the non-pathological batterer). There is empirical evidence, however, that suggests that both criminal history and severe violence toward their partners, although related, might be relatively independent (see for instance Boyle, O’Leary, Rosenbaum, & Hasset-Walker, 2008). This is especially important in studies with batterers conducted in prison, where reports of more severe violence are expected. For instance, using the typology of Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart, Walsh et al. (2010) found the same levels of physical violence for the generally violent/antisocial group (with longer criminal history) and the family-only violent group in a sample of civil-psychiatric patients. As it is discussed below, criminology literature on crime specialization provides theoretical arguments to anticipate that family-only batterers could be also involved in moderate to severe violence toward their partners.

Severity of Violence and Psychopathology

Both Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's (1994) and Cavanaugh and Gelles’ (2005) classifications of batterers seem to suggest a direct relationship between psychopathology and partner violence: one would expect moderate to severe psychopathology in individuals with severe violence toward their partners. There is empirical evidence about the role of psychopathology in the etiology of partner violence. In a longitudinal study of 543 participants belonging to a community sample followed over 20 years, Ehrensaft, Cohen, and Johnson (2006) found that men most seriously abusive toward their female partners also showed both antisocial and dramatic, emotionally dysregulated personality features (see also, Holtzworth-Munroe, Meehan, Herron, Rehman, & Stuart, 2003; Moffitt, Robins, & Caspi, 2001) and that Cluster B symptoms (narcissistic, antisocial, histrionic, and borderline) were the only significant personality predictors of increased risk of injury to a partner. The Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's batterer typology, as well as others, emphasizes the role of psychopathology to differentiate between groups of batterers. In their follow up of batterers, however, Holtzworth-Munroe et al. (2003) found that the generally violent and dysphoric/borderline groups were almost indistinguishable (see also Delsol, Margolin, & John, 2003; Holtzworth-Munroe & Meehan, 2004) and that level of psychopathy in these groups were similar (Holtzworth-Munroe, Meehan, Herron, Rehman, & Stuart, 2000; Huss, Covell, & Langhinrichsen-Rohling, 2006; Walsh et al., 2010). While the empirical evidence suggests that psychopathology could be on the onset of partner violence for some individuals, it might not allow to clearly distinguish between subgroups of batterers (dysphoric/borderline and generally violent/antisocial in Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's typology; moderate and high-risk batterer in Cavanaugh and Gelles’ typology).

In summary, the scientific literature on partner violence has provided empirical evidence about the heterogeneity of batterers and the most influential classifications of batterers distinguish between the less violent (family-only, low-risk offenders) and the more violent batterers (dysphoric/borderline and generally violent/antisocial; moderate and high-risk offenders). In these classifications, the batterer's severity of violence seems to be linked to the presence of an antisocial trajectory (i.e., criminal history) or severe psychopathology. In the case of imprisoned batterers, however, severity of violence tends to be present, leading to a lack of representativeness of the less violent batterer (family-only, low-risk offender). Also, as several researchers have pointed out, there seems to be an overlap between the more violent and psychologically distressed groups of batterers (Delsol et al., 2003; Holtzworth-Munroe et al., 2000; Holtzworth-Munroe et al., 2003; Holtzworth-Munroe & Meehan, 2004; Huss, Covell, & Langhinrichsen-Rohling, 2006; Walsh et al., 2010). These two circumstances limit the potential utility of the classical typologies when applied to the study of imprisoned male batterers and suggest the need for a classification that takes into account the characteristics of this population.

Generalist vs. Specialist Batterer

In the last few years, scholars have debated whether intimate partner violence might be considered different from other types of crimes. This debate has two sides: those who maintain that partner violence is a unique type of crime and those who support the idea that partner violence could be empirically indistinct from general crime. The accumulated empirical evidence in this topic is not conclusive. For instance, in their study of 2,124 offenders from a nationally representative sample of inmates from state and federal facilities in the U.S., Felson and Lane (2010) did not find empirical evidence supporting the idea that offenders who attacked partners were different from other offenders and could be regarded as typical offenders. Moffitt, Krueger, Caspi, & Fagan (2000) showed how general crime and partner violence were two different, although correlated, conceptual constructs. Using data from a longitudinal follow up for more than 20 years of 800 young adults, they found that many batterers also engaged in violence against non-intimates but the etiology of both types of violence seemed to be different as indicated by the existence of different correlates for each type of violence. For instance, low self-control (Constraint) predicted crime but not partner violence. As Baker, Metcalfe, and Jennings (2013) have recently pointed out, the versatility/specialization debate is both theoretical and methodological, where theories of general tendencies of antisocial behavior (Farrington, 2005; Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990) predict greater versatility than theories that assume different delinquent trajectories (Moffitt, 1993), or theories that underline the cultural (Dobash & Dobash, 1992) or intrafamilial (Gelles & Straus, 1979; Giles-Sims, 1983) origins of partner violence. The debate is also methodological in the sense that the new statistical methods tend to find greater levels of specialization (log-linear models, latent class analysis, quantile regression, etc.) than more traditional techniques such as factor analyses or simple comparisons of crime involvement. Also, the offender population under study and the type of source data used (official records vs. self-reports) might affect the degree of versatility/specialization found in the studies (Bouffard, Wright, Muftić, & Bouffard, 2008).

A generalist offender would commit different offenses on various occasions, with no inclination to pursue a specific criminal act or pattern of criminal acts whereas a specialist offender shows a greater tendency to repeat the same crime or offense over time (Baker et al., 2013). In the literature, specialization is seen as a type of consistency through a criminal career, which, indeed, might be relatively versatile. For the present study, a specialist male batterer is an offender whose only type of offenses are related to violence against his partner whereas a generalist male batterer is an offender with a criminal history of various types of offenses, including violence against his partner. This characterization presents an important difference with previous studies focusing on the degree of specialization among batterers (see Moffitt et al., 2000) and links the specialist batterer with the classical family-only violent batterer in Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's (1994) typology. Unlike typologies such as Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's, and Cavanaugh and Gelles’ (2005), where the family-only or low-risk offender are characterized by a low profile of violence (less severe and less frequent), in the present study the specialist batterer is not expected to use different levels of violence from the generalist batterer's. This would be in line with types of batterer such as the intimate terrorist in Johnson's (1995) typology (severe violence in the family whether unidirectional of mutual) or the Type II antisocial batterer in Gondolf's (1988) typology, where moderate to severe violence is expected inside the family but not outside the family.

The objective of the present study is to explore if there are substantial differences between these two types of imprisoned male batterers. To do so, the study followed Moffitt et al.’s (2000) suggestion when searching for differences among types of batterers. These authors suggested answering to two research questions: are these batterers the same or different people? Do they share the same or different correlates? The distribution of batterers across groups might help to answer the first question. For instance, if there were not a sizable number of members of any one group, the evidence for the existence of these two types of batterers would be weak. The analysis of the most common correlates of crime in general and partner violence in particular would help to answer the second question. If few or no statistically significant differences were found between the groups in a set of correlates, the evidence of true differences between the groups would be weak. To answer this second question the present study analyzes correlates in the individual, family, and community contexts that have been consistently linked to general crime and partner violence.

As individual characteristics, the study analyzes personality, sexist attitudes, and alcohol and substance dependence. There is compelling evidence that personality is linked to the onset of both criminal behavior (Blonigen & Krueger, 2007) and partner violence (Capaldi & Kim, 2007; Cavanaugh & Gelles, 2005), with a special incidence of Cluster B personality profile, which includes the narcissistic, antisocial, histrionic, and borderline personality (Ehrensaft et al., 2006). As for the role of attitudes toward women in partner violence, the literature is consistent in pointing at sexist attitudes as an important correlate of partner violence. This influence of sexist attitudes on partner violence is shared by several theoretical approaches, from theories that underline the importance of gender power disparity in society (feminist theories) to theories that emphasize the influence of cultural and family values (ecological) on the justification of abusive behavior toward female partners (see Ali & Naylor, 2013, for a review of studies).

The presence of alcohol and substance dependence has been consistently reported as an important correlate of both general aggression and partner violence (Wilkinson & Hamerschlag, 2005, for a review of studies) both at the time of a violent incident or as a distal correlate. In their meta-analysis of studies on the link between alcohol abuse and partner violence, Foran and O’Leary (2008) found a small to moderate effect size for the association between men alcohol abuse and partner violence. This relationship was greater in clinical samples and when severe alcohol problems were measured.

Family of origin has been proposed as a source of poor parenting skills, antisocial modeling, socioeconomic deprivation, and low attachment between the child and the parents that might be linked to violence in general (Farrington, 2003; LeBlanc, 2005; Thornberry, 2005) and partner violence in particular (Ehrensaft et al., 2003; Lussier, Farrington, & Moffitt, 2009). In the present study, family of origin climate and functioning represent the family context.

As for the community context, there are sound theoretical arguments as well as a vast array of empirical evidence on the ecology of partner violence (Lauritsen & Schaum, 2004; Pinchevsky & Wright, 2012; Van Wyk, Benson, Fox, & De Maris, 2003). According to Pinchevsky and Wright (2012), disadvantaged communities might influence partner violence in several ways. For instance, these communities may facilitate alienation and foster social isolation among citizens, which in turn influences the transmission of mainstream values that disapprove violence within couples. They also may hinder the formation and maintenance of social ties, leading to an increased vulnerability of residents. Also, they may intensify stress among couples, thus increasing the likelihood of partner violence. Thus, communities characterized by social disorder, and low levels of community integration and participation are expected to foster partner violence among its citizens. Community social disorder, community integration, and participation are variables representing the community context in the present study.

Finally, research has shown that offenders do not distribute homogeneously across sociodemographic characteristics. Thus, age, education, social class, and marital status as well as the delinquent trajectory of the offenders (age of onset of criminal behavior and age of first entry in the prison system) have been related to delinquent behavior and partner violence (Mazerolle & Maahs, 2000; Sabina, 2013; Van Wyk et al., 2003). In order to explore the profile of generalist and specialist offenders, we also included their sociodemographic characteristics.

MethodParticipants

Participants for this study were 110 men imprisoned in the Penitentiary of Villabona (Asturias, Spain), all convicted for violence against their female couples (gender violence) (see outcome variable section for a detailed description of participants). The study uses several sources of information. First, judicial and penitentiary reports were used to obtain information about the criminal history of participants. This included information about socio-demographic variables as well as the complete record of arrests and imprisonments for each participant. Second, different self-report measures were used to evaluate personal, family, and community contexts of participants.

Procedure

The researchers approached the penitentiary authorities and explained the study objectives in order to obtain permission to evaluate inmates on a set of variables. After permission was granted, participants who freely volunteered were individually evaluated.

VariablesOutcome variable

Based on officially reported information, participants were clustered into two groups: generalist (n = 86, 78%) and specialist batterers (n = 24, 22%). Generalist batterers had a varied criminal history, which included crimes other than partner violence, while specialist batterers had previous records, if any, of offenses exclusively related to intimate partner violence. Among the most frequent crimes and offenses committed by the group of generalist batterers were robbery (74.4%) and crime against public health (74.4%). Other less frequent crimes were economic offenses (34.9%) and homicide (17.4%). Among specialist batterers, two were convicted for homicide (8.3%) and the rest of them were convicted for assault and battery. In the case that a previous criminal record existed, the most frequent crime in this group was related to violation of no-contact orders.

Individual variablesPersonality

Participants completed the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III), which includes 175 true-false items used to detect personality disorders (Axis II) and major mental disorders (Axis I) through 24 subscales (Millon, 1997). Histrionic, Narcissistic, Antisocial, and Borderline scales scores were used for statistical comparison between groups. Number of items and Cronbach's alpha for each scale were as follows in the Spanish adaptation of the MCMI-III (Millon, Davis, & Millon, 2007): Histrionic (17 items, α = .80), Narcissistic (24 items, α = .70), Antisocial (17 items, α = .76), and Borderline (16 items, α = .82). Means and standard deviations for the scale in each group of batterers are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Means, Standard Deviations, and F Test on Individual Variables.

  GB (n = 86)  SB (n = 24)  F  p  η2 
Personality
Histrionic      0.23  .63  .00 
M  15.40  15.87       
SD  (4.26)  (3.76)       
Narcissistic      0.52  .47  .01 
M  14.34  14.95       
SD  (3.58)  (3.95)       
Antisocial      6.16  .02  .06 
M  14.12  11.17       
SD  (4.86)  (5.94)       
Borderline      5.89  .02  .05 
M  9.65  6.70       
SD  (5.40)  (4.50)       
Sexism
Hostile      5.89  .02  .05 
M  33.61  28.83       
SD  (6.47)  (7.48)       
Benevolent      0.15  .70  .00 
M  36.01  35.38       
SD  (6.97)  (7.59)       
Alcohol and substance dependence
Alcohol
M  8.80  9.04  0.04  .84  00 
SD  (4.90)  (5.23)       
Substance
M  13.32  8.67  14.40  .00  .12 
SD  (5.08)  (5.89)       

Note. df(1, 109). GB = generalist batterers; SB = specialist batterers.

Alcohol and substance dependence

Alcohol (15 items, α = .71) and Substance Dependence (14 items, α = .80) scales scores from the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III) were also obtained to compare between groups. Means and standard deviations for each scale and group are offered in Table 1.

Sexism

Participants completed the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (Glick & Fiske, 1996), which includes 22 items in two subscales: Hostile Sexism (‘Women seek to gain power by getting control over men’) and Benevolent Sexism (‘Women should be cherished and protected by men’). All of the items were rated on a five-point scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The Cronbach α of hostile and benevolent subscales in this study are acceptable at .84 and .79, respectively. Means and standard deviations for each scale and group are displayed in Table 1.

Family variablesFamily climate

The family Relationship Index is a 27-item, unidimensional measurement of the quality of social relationships in the family environment as measured by cohesion, expressiveness and conflict (Moos & Moos, 1994). The Cronbach α’s in this study were .88, .76, and .80 for cohesion, expressiveness, and conflict respectively. Means and standard deviations for the scale in each group of batterers are exposed in Table 2.

Table 2.

Means, Standard Deviations, and F Test on Family Variables.

  GB (n = 86)  SB (n = 24)  F  p  η2 
Family of origin: climate
Cohesion      0.16  .90  .00 
M  13.95  14.00       
SD  (1.55)  (1.88)       
Expressiveness      0.26  .87  .00 
M  14.15  14.21       
SD  (1.64)  (1.47)       
Conflict      8.01  .01  .07 
M  14.04  15.52       
SD  (2.31)  (1.75)       
Family of origin: functioning      2.21  .14  .02 
M  11.60  12.60       
SD  (2.99)  (2.34)       

Note .df(1, 109). GB = generalist batterers; SB = specialist batterers.

Family functioning

The Adaptability, Partnership, Growth, Affection, and Resolve scale (APGAR) was used to assess participant's perception of family functioning (Smilkstein, 1978). The measure consists of five 3-point scale items of family functioning: Adaptability, Partnership, Growth, Affection, and Resolve. Items responses range from 1 (hardly ever) to 3 (almost always). For this study, a summed up score scale was used. The Cronbach α was .86. Means and standard deviations for the scale in each group of batterers are presented in Table 2.

Community variablesCommunity social disorder

Community social disorder was measured with 3 items about the frequency of the following situations in the community (see Gracia & Herrero, 2006; Herrero & Gracia, 2005 for similar approaches): crime (fight with weapons, sexual aggressions, family violence, robbery, assaults, etc.), presence of drug traffic, and nightlife. Item responses raged on 5-point scale from (1) strongly disagree to (5) strongly agree (‘There are too much crime in my community – fight with weapons, sexual aggressions, family violence, robbery, assaults, etc.’). Internal consistency was adequate (Cronbach's α = .78). Means and standard deviations for the scale in each group of batterers are shown in Table 3.

Table 3.

Means, Standard Deviations, and F Test on Community Variables.

  GB (n = 86)  SB (n = 24)  F  p  η2 
Community social disorder      4.71  .03  .04 
M  7.94  5.95       
SD  (3.75)  (3.72)       
Community integration      4.47  .04  .04 
M  12.44  14.38       
SD  (3.87)  (3.24)       
Community participation      5.43  .02  .05 
M  12.11  14.90       
SD  (4.71)  (5.57)       

Note. df(1, 109). GB = generalist batterers; SB = specialist batterers.

Community integration and participation

The Community Integration and Community Participation Scales of the Perceived Community Support Questionnaire (Herrero & Gracia, 2007) were used. Community Integration (four items) and Community Participation (five items) measure sense of belonging and identification as well as participation in social activities in the community in a 5-point Likert scale. Both scales have shown adequate psychometric characteristics and predictive validity (Herrero & Gracia, 2007). The Cronbach α were.74 and .88 for the integration and participation scales, respectively. Means and standard deviations for the scale in each group of batterers are shown in Table 3.

Sociodemographic variables

Age was measured in years (M = 37.61, SD = 9.84). Marital status was distributed as follows: 47 were single (42.7%), 21 were married/living with couple (19.1%), 40 were divorced/legally separated (36.4%), and 2 were widowed (1.8%). Educational background was measured as 1 (elementary studies or lower), 2 (secondary studies – high school), or 3 (university studies). Nine participants had university studies (9.2%) and 35 participants had finished secondary studies (31.8%); most participants did not have secondary studies or lower (n = 66, 60%). Perceived social class was measured as 1 (low), 2 (middle), and 3 (high). Ninety three percent of participants perceived themselves as belonging to low or middle social class (n = 103). Information about age at first arrest and age at first imprisonment was obtained through official records. The average age at first arrest was 22.86 (SD = 11.12) and the average age at first imprisonment was 30.07 (SD = 10.71).

Data Analyses

Separate multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) were performed to estimate the effect of type of batterer on the dependent variables. Dependent variables were grouped in a theoretically meaningful way and statistically correlated variables for each MANOVA. Thus, Cluster B Personality, sexism, and alcohol and substance dependence variables were entered separately in three different MANOVAs. Family and community variables were entered in two separate MANOVAs. Univariate Analyses of Variance (ANOVA) were followed to each MANOVA to test for the means differences for each variable.

Results

Results show that all of the MANOVAs conducted were statistically significant, suggesting that generalists and specialists batterers scored differently on the individual, family and community variables (see Table 4). Looking at the partial effect as depicted by η2, the effects were greater in alcohol and substance dependence, community and family and Cluster B personality variables.

Table 4.

MANOVAs of Individual, Family, and Community Variables.

Variable  F  df  p  η2 
Individual
Personality  2.58  4.105  .04  .09 
Sexism  3.02  2.107  .05  .06 
Alcohol and substance dependence  8.28  2.107  .00  .14 
Family  2.46  4.105  .05  .09 
Community  3.98  3.106  .01  .10 
Individual Variables

As for the results of the univariate tests for the individual characteristics (see Table 1), looking first to the Cluster B variables, analyses showed that generalist batterers presented more antisocial and borderline characteristics than specialists batterers (F's ≥ 5.89, p's < .02) but similar levels of histrionic and narcissistic characteristics (F's ≤ 2.34, p's ≥ .47). As for the sexist attitudes, there were only differences between the groups in hostile sexism (F = 5.89, p = .02) with generalists batterers scoring higher than specialist batterers. Finally, there were also significant differences in substance dependence: generalist batterers showed greater substance dependence (F = 14.40, p < .001) than specialist batterers.

Family Variables

Specialist batterers showed greater levels of conflict in their family of origin than generalist batterers (F = 8.01, p = .01). Both generalist and specialist batterers presented similar levels on the positive characteristics of their family of origin, including cohesion (F = 0.16, p = .90), expressiveness (F = 0.26, p = .87) and the APGAR scores (F = 2.21, p = .14), which measure aspects such as adaptability, partnership, growth, affection and resolve (see Table 2).

Community Variables

The results for the univariate tests of community variables (see Table 3) showed that differences between groups were statistically different (F's ≥ 4.71, p's ≤ .04), indicating that generalist batterers seemed to come from more socially disordered communities as well as from communities in which they felt less integrated and participated to a lower extent.

Sociodemographic Variables

Generalist batterers were significantly younger (M = 36.48, SD = 9.38) than specialists batterers (M = 41.67, SD = 10.54), F = 5.50, df(1,109), p = .02, η2 = .05. Marital status of generalist batterers was statistically different, χ2(5, N = 110) = 13.13, p = .02, Cramer's V = .32, especially in the divorced/legally separated category (generalists 39%, specialists 58%; |z|= 3.8, p < .001) that was more frequent among specialist batterers. No statistical differences were found among type of batterer, social class, χ2(2, N = 110) = 0.68, p = .71, Cramer's V = .08, and educational background, χ2(5.38 (3, N = 110), p = .49, Cramer's V = .32. Generalists batterers showed an earlier onset of criminal behavior as can be seen both by the age at first arrest (generalist, M = 19.25, SD = 8.20; specialist, M = 35.50, SD = 10.89; F(1, 109) = 62.47, p < .001, η2 = .37), as well as the age at first imprisonment (generalist, M = 27.74, SD = 9.70; specialist M = 38.41, SD = 10.16; F(1, 109) = 22.45, p < .001, η2 = .19).

Discussion

In the present study a typology of imprisoned male batterer is proposed: the generalist vs. the specialist batterer. Drawing from both the literature on crime specialization and the typologies of male batterers, the criminal history of 110 imprisoned male batterers was used to distinguish between the generalist and specialist batterer. The group of generalist batterers was formed by inmates with a criminal history of various types of offenses, including violence against his partner. The group of specialist batterers was formed by those inmates whose offenses were always related to violence against his partner. Although both types of batterers have characteristics in common with other types of batterers described in the literature, they also present important differences. The generalist batterer is closer to the generally violent/antisocial batterer in Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's (1994) typology and the high-risk offender in Cavanaugh and Gelles’ (2005) classification. The specialist batterer has not a long criminal history, in line with the family-only and low-risk batterers of Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's and Cavanaugh and Gelles’ classifications. They do not necessarily present, however, the low-violence profile of these typologies. In this sense, they would be closer to the intimate terrorist in Johnson's (1995) typology or the Type II antisocial batterer in Gondolf's (1988) typology where moderate to severe violence seems to be limited to the family environment.

Two research questions regarding these groups guided the present research: 1) are they the same people? 2) do they share the same correlates? As for the first question, the distribution of batterers across groups suggests that although most offenders were in the generalist group (n = 86, 75%) there were also a sizable number of batterers with no other criminal record or with a criminal history consistently related to violence toward his partner (n = 24, 25%). This finding seems to give support to the idea that although crime generalization is the most typical profile of offenders, there is also a certain degree of specialists among them (Baker et al., 2013; DeLisi et al., 2011; Felson & Lane, 2010).

Next, we explored the sociodemographic, individual, family, and community characteristics for each group. The generalist batterers not only present a longer and more varied criminal history but also an earlier onset in his criminal activity. Most generalist batterers had committed offenses related to drug use and drug traffic (crimes against public health, 74%) and robbery (74%), but there were also murderers (17%) in this group. Specialist batterers presented a shorter criminal history and specifically related to partner violence, mainly for violation of no-contact orders. These results suggest that, indeed, there is a different criminal trajectory in each group: the generalist batterer is younger and with an earlier onset in his criminal activities, which by definition is more varied. This finding would be consistent with research showing that the age of delinquency onset is inversely related to the number of offenses (Mazerolle, Burton, Cullen, Evans, & Payne, 2000; McGloin, Sullivan, Piquero, & Pratt, 2007; Piquero, Paternoster, Mazerolle, Brame, & Dean, 1999) and that younger and persistent offenders tend to have a more varied criminal history (Arce, Fariña, & Vázquez, 2011), including partner violence (Mazerolle & Maahs, 2000).

As for the differences in their individual characteristics, the specialist batterer seems to present a lower profile of psychopathology than the generalist batterer, specifically in Cluster B personality aspects such as antisocial and borderline personality. This would be in line with the family-only batterer (Holtzworth-Munroe & Stuart, 1994) or the low-risk offender (Cavanaugh & Gelles, 2005) proposed in the literature. Unlike these types of batterers, however, the specialist batterer also presents moderate to severe (even lethal) violence. In fact, there were two murderers among this group of specialist batterers, which challenges the classical notion that the family-only batterer usually presents a low level of partner violence. Specialist and generalist batterers showed no significant differences in other Cluster B characteristics such as histrionic or narcissistic personality that have been related to partner violence (Ehrensaft et al., 2006; García-Jiménez, Godoy-Fernández, Llor-Esteban, & Ruiz-Hernández, 2014; Torres, Lemos-Giráldez, & Herrero, 2013), and batterer treatment efficacy (Novo, Fariña, Seijo, & Arce, 2012).

Generalist batterers showed greater levels of substance dependence than the specialist batterers and similar levels of alcohol dependence. The fact that most of generalist batterers of the study had been convicted for crimes related to drug traffic (around 75% of generalist batterers), which tend to be linked to drug use and abuse, might explain this finding. Also, alcohol and substance dependence has been regarded to partner violence (Wilkinson & Hamerschlag, 2005). Both alcohol and substance dependence may exert their influence at the social, economic, and relational level, thereby increasing the stress in the relationship and the likelihood of partner violence.

Regarding their sexist attitudes, the generalist batterer showed greater levels of hostile sexism but similar levels of benevolent sexism when compared to the specialist batterers. Research on ambivalent sexism has shown that those who are high in hostile sexism are more tolerant of intimate partner violence (Glick, Sakalli-Ugurlu, Ferreira, & Souza, 2002) while benevolent sexism have been related to victim-blaming attitudes (Viki & Abrams, 2002) toward women. These attitudes serve as an anchorage that guides the information interpretation, supporting preconceptions against women (Fariña, Arce, & Novo, 2002), which contributes to sustain offending (Maruna, 2004).

As for their family of origin correlates, family functioning was different in each group. Specialist batterers scored significantly higher in conflict in family of origin, indicating that they portrayed their family of origin as a context were family members more openly expressed anger and conflict than in the case of generalist batterers. This finding would be consistent both with theories that emphasize the intrafamilial origins of partner violence (Gelles, 2007) and with the empirical evidence linking exposure to family violence and partner violence in adult life. Longitudinal research has found that exposure to violence between parents is a consistent predictor of partner violence in adult life (Ehrensaft et al., 2003; Lavoie et al., 2002; Simons, Lin, & Gordon, 1998). In this sense, Lussier et al. (2009) have found evidence supporting that family environment increases the risk of partner violence, mainly by fostering the development of antisocial behavior and neuropsychological deficits (see also Capaldi & Clark, 1998). This would be in line with the antisocial/generally violent batterer in Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart's (1994) and the high-risk offender in Cavanaugh and Gelles’ (2005) typologies. Alternatively, the existence of moderate to severe violence toward female partner in the group of specialist batterers would illustrate the role that conflictive family environments might have on the development of patterns of aggression in intimate relationships, with no further need of development of antisocial behavior. This would explain the existence of types such as Johnson's (1995) intimate terrorist or Gondolf's (1999) Type II antisocial batterer, where moderate to severe violence is expected inside the family but not outside the family.

Besides this debate about the direct or indirect influence of family functioning on partner violence (through antisocial behavior and neuropsychological deficits), our findings indicate that conflicts in the family of origin might be a key influence on partner violence in the case of the specialist batterer. No differences were found regarding the more positive aspects of family functioning such as cohesion, expressiveness, adaptability, partnership, growth, affection, and resolve. The fact that these types of batterers shared most of the family correlates is compatible with the idea shared by many scholars that both the more general antisocial trajectory and the specialization in partner violence are related to the existence of dysfunctional families of origin (Ehrensaft et al., 2003; Farrington, 2003; Gelles, 2007; LeBlanc, 2005; Lussier et al., 2009; Thornberry, 2005).

Finally, it seemed that these two groups came from different communities or residential areas: the generalist batterer described his community as more socially disordered and showed lower levels of integration and participation toward it. As Pinchevsky and Wright (2012) concluded in their extensive review of studies about the impact of neighborhoods on intimate partner violence, disordered neighborhoods have been consistently linked to higher risk of partner violence while those characterized by the existence of support ties are more protected from such violence (see also Gracia & Herrero, 2007; Gracia, Herrero, Lila & Fuente, 2009; Herrero & Gracia, 2005). Our findings seem to support this claim, especially in the case of the generalist batterer with a more varied criminal history. The lower levels of participation and integration in a community and the higher levels of community social disorder exemplify a well-studied path between neighborhood conditions and both general and partner violence (Lauritsen & Schaum, 2004; Markowitz, Bellair, Liska, & Liu, 2001; Van Wyk et al., 2003). While generalist batterers in our study seem to fit well into this explanation, our findings suggest that community correlates were not so important in the case of the specialist batterer. Thus, the specialist batterer lived under community conditions of lower social disorder and higher integration and participation with apparently less influence on partner violence. Other research has found levels of partner violence under low disordered neighborhood conditions. For instance, Van Wyk et al. (2003) found that intimate partner violence may increase its likelihood even at levels of low social disorganization if certain circumstances are present (i.e., lack of contacts for women).

Overall, the study findings help to portray a generalist batterer, which shows higher levels of psychopathology, substance dependence, and sexist attitudes. Also, their communities seem to be more socially disordered and their levels of community participation and integration are lower. They also present a longer and more varied criminal history with an earlier onset, both in terms of the age of first arrest as well as the age of first imprisonment. The specialist batterer, who represents a minority of the batterers analyzed in the present research, presents a profile of lower psychopathology, substance dependence, sexism, and community social disorder and a profile of higher levels of conflict in the family of origin along with higher levels of community integration and participation.

The study presents several potential limitations, however. First, most information regarding the criminal history of inmates was collected through official records. Previous research has found that specialization of offenders is more evident when using official records instead of self-reported information, although with some exceptions (see Bouffard et al., 2008 for a review of studies). If this were the case, it would be possible that some of the specialist batterers in our study had committed other than partner violence-related offenses that were not officially reported. The significant differences found in the criminal history of specialist and generalist batterers seem to suggest, however, that there are true differences among them (i.e., later onset of criminal activity in specialist batterers). With the present data we cannot rule completely out other alternative explanations based on the existence of undetected criminal activity in participants of the study. Further research using both self-reported as well as officially reported information should clarify this point.

Second, participants of the study might not be representative of the convicted batterer population, so generalization of results is not warranted. Again, the clear differences in both the onset of criminal activity and the age of first imprisonment seems to suggest that these two groups exist in the population of imprisoned male batterers although we should be cautious about the distribution of batterers across groups. In our study, three out of every four batterers belonged to the generalist group, in contrast with Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart (1994) suggestion that both generalist/antisocial and dysphoric/borderline batterers would account for almost 50% of the population of batterers. Further research with representative samples should provide more accurate estimates of the true distribution of generalist and specialist batterers.

And last, but not least, the definition of specialist batterers used in this study might be too restricted, potentially leading to low group stability across time. In this sense, the specialist batterer could belong to the generalist group if a non-partner violence-related offense is committed. It seems clear, however, that for most specialist offenders their criminal history is limited to this type of offense, as seen by their late onset of criminal activity and age at first imprisonment. Future research focusing on the trajectory of batterers could add relevant information about the formation of these two groups of batterers.

Conflict of Interest

The authors of this article declare no conflict of interest.

Financial Support

Support for this research was provided by grant from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (FEM2012-30659).

References
[Ali and Naylor, 2013]
P.A. Ali, P.B. Naylor.
Intimate partner violence: A narrative review of the feminist, social and ecological explanations for its causation.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 18 (2013), pp. 611-619
[Arce et al., 2011]
R. Arce, F. Fariña, M.J. Vázquez.
Grado de competencia social y comportamientos antisociales delictivos y no delictivos en menores [Social competence and delinquent, antisocial and non-deviant behavior in adolescents].
Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 43 (2011), pp. 473-486
[Baker et al., 2013]
T. Baker, Ch.F. Metcalfe, W.G. Jennings.
What are the odds? Predicting specialization in offending over the life course.
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 40 (2013), pp. 909-932
[Bender and Roberts, 2007]
K. Bender, A.R. Roberts.
Battered women versus male batterer typologies: Same or different based on evidence-based studies?.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 12 (2007), pp. 519-530
[Blonigen and Krueger, 2007]
D.M. Blonigen, R.F. Krueger.
Personality and violence: The unifying role of structural models of personality.
The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior, pp. 288-305 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816840.014
[Bouffard et al., 2008]
L.B. Bouffard, K.A. Wright, R. Muftić, J.A. Bouffard.
Gender differences in specialization in intimate partner violence: Comparing the gender symmetry and violent resistance perspectives.
Justice Quarterly, 25 (2008), pp. 570-594
[Boyle et al., 2008]
D.J. Boyle, K.D. O’Leary, A. Rosenbaum, C. Hasset-Walker.
Differentiating between generally and partner-only violent subgroups: Lifetime antisocial behavior, family of origin violence, and impulsivity.
Journal of Family Violence, 23 (2008), pp. 47-55
[Capaldi and Clark, 1998]
D.M. Capaldi, S. Clark.
Prospective family predictors of aggression toward female partners for at-risk young men.
Developmental Psychology, 34 (1998), pp. 1175-1188
[Capaldi and Kim, 2007]
D.M. Capaldi, H.K. Kim.
Typological approaches to violence in couples: A critique and alternative conceptual approach.
Clinical Psychology Review, 27 (2007), pp. 253-265
[Cavanaugh and Gelles, 2005]
M.M. Cavanaugh, R.J. Gelles.
The utility of male domestic violence offender typologies: New directions for research, policy, and practice.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20 (2005), pp. 155-166
[Cunha and Gonçalves, 2013]
O. Cunha, R.A. Gonçalves.
Intimate partner violence offenders: Generating a data-based typology of batterers and implications for treatment.
The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 5 (2013), pp. 131-139
[DeLisi et al., 2011]
M. DeLisi, K.M. Beaver, K.A. Wright, J.P. Wright, M.G. Vaughn, Ch.R. Trulson.
Criminal specialization revisited: A simultaneous quantile regression approach.
American Journal of Criminal Justice, 36 (2011), pp. 73-92
[Delsol et al., 2003]
C. Delsol, G. Margolin, R.S. John.
A typology of martially violent men and correlates of violence in a community sample.
Journal of Marriage and Family, 65 (2003), pp. 635-651
[Dobash and Dobash, 1992]
R.E. Dobash, R.P. Dobash.
Women, violence and social change.
[Ehrensaft et al., 2003]
M.K. Ehrensaft, P. Cohen, J. Brown, E. Smiles, H. Chen, J.G. Johnson.
Intergenerational transmission of partner violence: A 20-year prospective study.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71 (2003), pp. 741-753
[Ehrensaft et al., 2006]
M.K. Ehrensaft, P. Cohen, J.G. Johnson.
Development of personality disorder symptoms and the risk for partner violence.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115 (2006), pp. 474-483
[Fariña et al., 2002]
F. Fariña, R. Arce, M. Novo.
Heurístico de anclaje en las decisiones judiciales [Anchorage in judicial decision making].
Psicothema, 14 (2002), pp. 39-46
[Farrington, 2003]
D.P. Farrington.
Developmental and life-course criminology: Key theoretical and empirical issues.
Criminology, 41 (2003), pp. 221-255
[Farrington, 2005]
D.P. Farrington.
The integrated cognitive antisocial potential (ICAP) theory.
Integrated developmental and life-course theories of crime, pp. 1-14
[Felson and Lane, 2010]
R.B. Felson, K.J. Lane.
Does violence involving women and intimate partners have a special etiology?.
Criminology, 48 (2010), pp. 321-338
[Foran and O’Leary, 2008]
H.M. Foran, K.D. O’Leary.
Alcohol and intimate partner violence: A meta-analytic review.
Clinical Psychology Review, 28 (2008), pp. 1222-1234
[García-Jiménez et al., 2014]
J.J. García-Jiménez, C. Godoy-Fernández, B. Llor-Esteban, J.A. Ruiz-Hernández.
Differential profile in partner aggressors: Prison vs. mandatory community intervention programs.
The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 6 (2014), pp. 69-77
[Gelles, 2007]
R.J. Gelles.
Family violence.
The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior, pp. 403-417 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816840.020
[Gelles and Straus, 1979]
R.J. Gelles, M.A. Straus.
Determinants of violence in the family: Toward a theoretical integration.
pp. 549-581
[Giles-Sims, 1983]
J. Giles-Sims.
Wife battering: A systems theory approach.
Guilford Press, (1983),
[Glick and Fiske, 1996]
P. Glick, S.T. Fiske.
The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 (1996), pp. 491-512
[Glick et al., 2002]
P. Glick, N. Sakalli-Ugurlu, M.C. Ferreira, M.A. de Souza.
Ambivalent sexism and attitudes toward wife abuse in Turkey and Brazil.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26 (2002), pp. 292-297
[Gondolf, 1988]
E.W. Gondolf.
Who are those guys? Toward a behavioral typology of batterers.
Violence and Victims, 3 (1988), pp. 187-203
[Gondolf, 1999]
E.W. Gondolf.
Characteristics of court-mandated batterers in four cities: Diversity and dichotomies.
Violence against Women, 5 (1999), pp. 1277-1293
[Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990]
M.R. Gottfredson, T. Hirschi.
A general theory of crime.
Stanford University Press, (1990),
[Gracia and Herrero, 2006]
E. Gracia, J. Herrero.
Perceived neighborhood social disorder and residents’ attitudes toward reporting child physical abuse.
Child Abuse & Neglect, 30 (2006), pp. 357-365
[Gracia and Herrero, 2007]
E. Gracia, J. Herrero.
Perceived neighborhood social disorder and attitudes towards reporting domestic violence against women.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22 (2007), pp. 737-752
[Gracia et al., 2009]
E. Gracia, J. Herrero, M. Lila, A. Fuente.
Perceived neighborhood social disorder and attitudes toward domestic violence against women among Latin-American immigrants.
The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 1 (2009), pp. 25-43
[Hamberger et al., 1996]
L.K. Hamberger, J.M. Lohr, D. Bonge, D.F. Tonlin.
A large sample empirical typology of male spouse abusers and its relationship to dimensions of abuse.
Violence and Victims, 11 (1996), pp. 277-292
[Herrero and Gracia, 2005]
J. Herrero, E. Gracia.
Perceived frequency of domestic violence against women and neighborhood social disorder.
Psychological Reports, 97 (2005), pp. 712-716
[Herrero and Gracia, 2007]
J. Herrero, E. Gracia.
Measuring perceived community support: Factorial structure, longitudinal invariance and predictive validity of the PCSQ (Perceived Community Support Questionnaire).
Journal of Community Psychology, 35 (2007), pp. 197-217
[Holtzworth-Munroe and Meehan, 2004]
A. Holtzworth-Munroe, J.C. Meehan.
Typologies of men who are maritally violent scientific and clinical implications.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19 (2004), pp. 1369-1389
[Holtzworth-Munroe et al., 2000]
A. Holtzworth-Munroe, J.C. Meehan, K. Herron, U. Rehman, G.L. Stuart.
Testing the Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart (1994) batterer typology.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68 (2000), pp. 1000-1019
[Holtzworth-Munroe et al., 2003]
A. Holtzworth-Munroe, J.C. Meehan, K. Herron, U. Rehman, G.L. Stuart.
Do subtypes of maritally violent men continue to differ over time?.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71 (2003), pp. 728-740
[Holtzworth-Munroe and Stuart, 1994]
A. Holtzworth-Munroe, G.L. Stuart.
Typologies of male batterers: Three subtypes and the differences among them.
Psychological Bulletin, 116 (1994), pp. 476-497
[Huss et al., 2006]
M.T. Huss, C.N. Covell, J. Langhinrichsen-Rohling.
Clinical implications for the assessment and treatment of antisocial and psychopathic domestic violence perpetrators.
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 13 (2006), pp. 59-85
[Johnson, 1995]
M.P. Johnson.
Patriarchal terrorism and common couple violence: Two forms of violence against women.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57 (1995), pp. 283-294
[Lauritsen and Schaum, 2004]
J.L. Lauritsen, R.J. Schaum.
The social ecology of violence against women.
Criminology, 42 (2004), pp. 323-356
[Lavoie et al., 2002]
F. Lavoie, M. Hebert, R. Tremblay, F. Vitaro, L. Vezina, P. McDuff.
History of family dysfunction and perpetration of dating violence by adolescent boys: A longitudinal study.
Journal of Adolescent Health, 30 (2002), pp. 375-383
[LeBlanc, 2005]
M. LeBlanc.
An integrative personal control theory of deviant behavior: Answers to contemporary empirical and theoretical developmental criminology issues.
Integrated life course and developmental theories of offending, pp. 125-163
[Lussier et al., 2009]
P. Lussier, D.P. Farrington, T.E. Moffitt.
Is the antisocial child father of the abusive man?. A 40-year prospective longitudinal study on the developmental antecedents of intimate partner violence.
Criminology, 47 (2009), pp. 741-780
[Markowitz et al., 2001]
F.E. Markowitz, P.E. Bellair, A.E. Liska, J.H. Liu.
Extending social disorganization theory: Modeling the relationships between cohesion, fear, and disorder.
Criminology, 38 (2001), pp. 205-218
[Maruna, 2004]
S. Maruna.
Desistance and explanatory style: A new direction in the psychology of reform.
Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 20 (2004), pp. 184-200
[Mazerolle et al., 2000]
P. Mazerolle, V.S. Burton, F.T. Cullen, D. Evans, G.L. Payne.
Strain, anger, and delinquent adaptations: Specifying general strain theory.
Journal of Criminal Justice, 28 (2000), pp. 89-101
[Mazerolle and Maahs, 2000]
P. Mazerolle, J. Maahs.
General strain and delinquency: An alternative examination of conditioning influences.
Justice Quarterly, 17 (2000), pp. 753-778
[McGloin et al., 2007]
J.M. McGloin, C.J. Sullivan, A.R. Piquero, T.C. Pratt.
Local life circumstances and offending specialization/versatility: Comparing opportunity and propensity models.
Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 44 (2007), pp. 321-346
[Millon, 1997]
T. Millon.
Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III) manual.
2nd ed., National Computer Systems, (1997),
[Millon et al., 2007]
T. Millon, R. Davis, C. Millon.
MCMI-III. Manual.
TEA Ediciones, (2007),
[Moffitt, 1993]
T.E. Moffitt.
Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy.
Psychological Review, 100 (1993), pp. 674-701
[Moffitt et al., 2000]
T.E. Moffitt, R.F. Krueger, A. Caspi, J. Fagan.
Partner abuse and general crime: how are they the same? How are they different?.
Criminology, 38 (2000), pp. 199-232
[Moffitt et al., 2001]
T.E. Moffitt, R.B. Robins, A. Caspi.
A couples analysis of partner abuse with implications for abuse prevention policy.
Criminology and Public Policy, 1 (2001), pp. 5-36
[Moos and Moos, 1994]
R.H. Moos, B.S. Moos.
Family Environment Scale Manual: Development, applications research.
3rd ed., Consulting Psychologist Press, (1994),
[Novo et al., 2012]
M. Novo, F. Fariña, D. Seijo, R. Arce.
Assessment of a community rehabilitation programme in convicted male intimate-partner violent offenders.
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 12 (2012), pp. 219-234
[Pinchevsky and Wright, 2012]
G.M. Pinchevsky, E.M. Wright.
The impact of neighborhoods on intimate partner violence and victimization.
Trauma Violence Abuse, 13 (2012), pp. 112-132
[Piquero et al., 1999]
A. Piquero, R.P. Paternoster, P. Mazerolle, R. Brame, Ch.W. Dean.
Onset age and offense specialization.
Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 36 (1999), pp. 275-299
[Sabina, 2013]
Ch. Sabina.
Individual and national level associations between economic deprivation and partner violence among college students in 31 national settings.
Aggressive Behavior, 39 (2013), pp. 247-256
[Simons et al., 1998]
R.L. Simons, H. Lin, L.C. Gordon.
Socialization in the family of origin and male dating violence: A prospective study.
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60 (1998), pp. 467-478
[Smilkstein, 1978]
G. Smilkstein.
The family APGAR: A proposal for family function test and its use by physicians.
Journal of Family Practice, 6 (1978), pp. 1231-1239
[Thornberry, 2005]
T.P. Thornberry.
Explaining multiple patterns of offending across the life course and across generations.
Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Sciences, 602 (2005), pp. 156-195
[Torres et al., 2013]
A. Torres, S. Lemos-Giráldez, J. Herrero.
Violencia hacia la mujer: Características psicológicas y de personalidad de los hombres que maltratan a su pareja.
Anales de Psicología, 29 (2013), pp. 9-18
[Van Wyk et al., 2003]
J. Van Wyk, M.L. Benson, G.L. Fox, A. De Maris.
Detangling individual-, partner-, and community-level correlates of partner violence.
Crime and Delinquency, 49 (2003), pp. 412-438
[Viki and Abrams, 2002]
G.T. Viki, D. Abrams.
But she was unfaithful: Benevolent sexism and reactions to rape victims that violate traditional gender role expectations.
Sex Roles, 47 (2002), pp. 289-293
[Walsh et al., 2010]
Z. Walsh, M.T. Swogger, B. O’Connor, Y.C. Shonbrun, M.T. Shea, G.L. Stuart.
Subtypes of partner violence perpetrators among male and female civil psychiatric patients.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119 (2010), pp. 563-574
[Wilkinson and Hamerschlag, 2005]
D.L. Wilkinson, S.J. Hamerschlag.
Situational determinants in intimate partner violence.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10 (2005), pp. 333-361
Copyright © 2016. Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid
Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo