metricas
covid
Buscar en
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology
Toda la web
Inicio International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology Effects of human concurrent aerobic and resistance training on cognitive health:...
Información de la revista
Visitas
116
Vol. 25. Núm. 1. (En progreso)
(enero - marzo 2025)
Acceso a texto completo
Effects of human concurrent aerobic and resistance training on cognitive health: A systematic review with meta-analysis
Visitas
116
Mingyang Zhanga,1,∗∗, Wangfan Fanga, Jiahong Wangb,
a School of Sport Science, Jishou University, Jishou, China
b Soochow University Think Tank, Soochow University, Suzhou, China
Este artículo ha recibido
Información del artículo
Resumen
Texto completo
Bibliografía
Descargar PDF
Estadísticas
Figuras (6)
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Tablas (4)
Table 1. Eligibility criteria for meta-analysis.
Tablas
Table 2. Characteristics of the included studies.
Tablas
Table 3. Moderator analysis of effects of concurrent training on cognitive function outcomes.
Tablas
Table 4. Meta-regression analysis for potential heterogeneity.
Tablas
Mostrar másMostrar menos
Material adicional (1)
Abstract
Background

The rising prevalence of cognitive decline and neurodegenerative diseases, projected to affect 150 million individuals by 2050, highlights the urgent need to enhance neurocognitive health. While both aerobic and resistance training are recognized as effective strategies, their combined effects on cognition remain underexplored.

Objective

This study aimed to determine if concurrent aerobic and resistance training (CT) is effective in enhancing cognitive function.

Methods

Seven English and three Chinese databases were searched from inception to August 2024. Randomised controlled trials (RCTs) examining the effects of CT on global cognition across diverse populations were included. A meta-analysis was performed using a random-effects model in R and Stata, supplemented by subgroup and meta-regression analyses to explore variability.

Results

The meta-analysis included 35 RCTs with 5,734 participants, revealing a positive effect of CT on global cognition (g = 0.32, 95% CI: 0.17–0.46, p < 0.001). Notably, older adults (≥65 years) exhibited greater cognitive benefits (g = 0.33; 95% CI: 0.14–0.51, p < 0.05) compared to younger populations. Significant effects were also observed in clinical populations (g = 0.28; 95% CI: 0.11–0.46, p < 0.001). Exercise frequency and duration positively influenced outcomes, with medium-length interventions (13–26 weeks) demonstrating significant effects (g = 0.21; 95% CI: 0.05–0.37, p = 0.011).

Conclusion

The findings indicate that CT significantly enhances cognitive health, particularly in older adults and clinical populations. Prioritizing strength training, implementing short- to medium-term interventions (4–26 weeks), and maintaining session durations of 30–60 minutes are crucial for optimizing cognitive benefits.

Keywords:
Exercise
Concurrent training
Cognition
Mild cognitive impairment
Older adults
Health
Texto completo
Introduction

Cognitive function—spanning memory, attention, executive function, and processing speed—is essential across the lifespan, influencing school readiness (Blair, 2002; Diamond & Lee, 2011), academic achievement (Rohde & Thompson, 2007; Welsh et al., 2010), job performance (Lang et al., 2010), and overall health in later life (Ludyga et al., 2020). However, aging and neurodegenerative diseases (e.g., Alzheimer's disease) threaten neurocognitive health, defined as the optimal state of these cognitive domains that enable individuals to maintain independence and quality of life (Stillman et al., 2020). Indeed, healthy aging encompasses not only the absence of disease or disability but also the ability to maintain functional capabilities in older age (Beard et al., 2016; Organization, 2015; Rowe & Kahn, 1997). Given the dynamic and nonlinear nature of cognitive trajectories throughout the lifespan (Turrini et al., 2023), it is crucial to differentiate between synergistic adaptations and natural cognitive growth when evaluating the benefits (Robinson et al., 2023). Critically, cognitive health extends beyond mere enhancement; it encompasses both the improvement of current capacities and the delay of age- or pathology-related decline (Deckers et al., 2024; Kaliman et al., 2011; Stern, 2002). This dual perspective aligns with the World Health Organization's holistic definition: “the dynamic maintenance of cognitive resilience to support well-being and societal engagement across the lifespan (Organization, 2021)”. Moreover, mental disorders, such as depression and schizophrenia, are increasingly recognized as risk factors for accelerated cognitive decline (Kandola et al., 2019). These conditions are associated with dysregulated neurotrophic factors, chronic inflammation, and impaired neurogenesis, which may exacerbate cognitive deficits (Pereira et al., 2007). However, exercise-induced brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) elevation (Kim et al., 2019) and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis modulation (Schuch, Vancampfort, Sui, et al., 2016) may concurrently alleviate depressive symptoms and enhance memory consolidation—a hypothesis supported by trials integrating physical exercise with cognitive-behavioral therapy (Biazus-Sehn et al., 2020; Nuechterlein et al., 2018). Understanding the interplay between mental health and exercise-induced cognitive benefits is critical for optimising programs. Therefore, the current question of how to achieve successful aging is attracting the attention of accumulating healthcare providers and policymakers (Jia et al., 2020).

Lifelong engagement in physical activity has emerged as a promising strategy to delay (Paillard, 2015; Zimmerman et al., 2014), prevent (Sujkowski et al., 2022; Turrini et al., 2023), or even reverse (Kaliman et al., 2011; Xu et al., 2021) cognitive decline (Stillman et al., 2020). Conversely, sedentary behavior was seen as a major threat for physical and cognitive health (Piercy et al., 2018; Zou et al., 2024). Exercise, characterized by its accessibility and minimal side effects, offers benefits that beyond mere physical fitness (Larson & Bruce, 1987; Penedo & Dahn, 2005). Recent research highlights the growing interest in exercise programs aimed at improving cognitive health (A'Naja et al., 2024; Ren et al., 2024). Among the various types of exercise, aerobic/endurance training (AET) and strength/resistance training (SRT) have demonstrated significant benefits for psychophysiological health (Al-Mhanna et al., 2024; Al-Mhanna et al., 2023; Batrakoulis & Fatouros, 2022), particularly in older adults and clinical populations (Ballesteros et al., 2024; Chow et al., 2021; Erickson et al., 2019; Gallardo-Gómez et al., 2022). AET has been shown to enhance cardiovascular health and cognitive function, reducing risks related to brain health and improving executive control (Al-Mhanna et al., 2023; Batrakoulis et al., 2022). On the other hand, SRT contributes to improved muscle function and supports brain adaptability (McArdle et al., 2010; Nagamatsu et al., 2012). Furthermore, concurrent training (CT), which combines aerobic and resistance training, has emerged as a promising approach due to its potential for delivering combined benefits (Al-Mhanna et al., 2024). Both AET and SRT are characterized by automatic repetition, with higher metabolic energy and lower cognitive engagement (Ludyga et al., 2022; Netz, 2019; Tomporowski & Pesce, 2019; Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013). Recent studies (Meijer et al., 2020) emphasizing the impact of cognitively demanding exercise that involve mental involvement (such as mind-body exercises and skill training) might introduce confounding factors in the cognitive outcome. In contrast, exploring the ‘black box’ of how energy demand-dominant exercise improve cognition is far more intriguing.

A substantial body of evidence-based research investigates the effects (Cammisuli et al., 2017; Ren et al., 2023) and mechanisms (Liang et al., 2021; Tari et al., 2019) by which both AET (Cammisuli et al., 2017; Stern et al., 2019) and SRT (Coelho-Junior et al., 2022; Mavros et al., 2017) enhance cognitive function. The simplicity of AET has led to its more widespread use in research settings for enhancing cognitive health compared to SRT (Ciria et al., 2023; de Asteasu et al., 2017; Song et al., 2018). Most large-scale meta-analyses examining the relationship between exercise training and cognitive function (Chen et al., 2020; Haverkamp et al., 2020; Northey et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2023) have included more studies on AET than on SRT. Specifically, both AET and SRT improve global cognition (Han et al., 2023), attention (Dunsky et al., 2017; Hacker et al., 2020), memory (Babaei et al., 2013; Cassilhas et al., 2012; Erickson et al., 2011; Makino et al., 2021), information processing speed (Eckardt et al., 2020; Sandroff et al., 2015), cognitive flexibility (Netz et al., 2007; Segabinazi et al., 2020), and executive functions, including working memory (Ludyga et al., 2022; Pontifex et al., 2009), planning (Y. K. Chang, P. W. Ku, et al., 2012; Y. K. Chang, C. Y. Pan, et al., 2012; Hung et al., 2013), and inhibitory control (Amatriain‐Fernández et al., 2021; Ludyga et al., 2018; Wilke et al., 2020). The acute and chronic effects of AET benefit cognitive health across various age groups, including children and adolescents (Davis et al., 2007; Harveson et al., 2016; Robinson et al., 2023), young and middle-aged adults (Dunsky et al., 2017; Stern et al., 2019; Stroth et al., 2009), and older adults (Best et al., 2015; Garcia-Soto et al., 2013), as well as healthy (Nouchi & Kawashima, 2014) and diverse clinical populations (Cerrillo‐Urbina et al., 2015; McDonnell et al., 2011; Papatsimpas et al., 2023; Zhang et al., 2022). Conversely, both acute and chronic studies indicate that SRT improves cognitive function across age groups; however, the effects of exercise on attention improvement and acute responses in adolescents are not fully understood. Furthermore, inconsistencies exist in findings related to the global cognitive benefits of AET (Young et al., 2015), improvements in global cognition (J. R. Ruiz et al., 2015), memory (Cavalcante et al., 2020), and inhibition (Chang & Etnier, 2009) associated with SRT, as well as the effects on working memory from both exercise modalities (Landrigan et al., 2020; Zhidong et al., 2021). Scientific evidence on the relationship between exercise and cognition has steadily increased over the past half-century. Current research explores the dose-response relationship (Engeroff et al., 2019; McCartney et al., 2021) and optimal exercise prescriptions (Huang et al., 2022) in depth. However, existing literature primarily emphasizes the individual effects of these exercise modalities, overlooking potential synergistic or interference effects from their combined application.

The concept of “interference effects” in CT has been extensively discussed at the physiological level (Coffey & Hawley, 2017; Fyfe et al., 2014; Huiberts et al., 2024; Lundberg et al., 2022; Wadsworth et al., 2022). However, its implications for neurocognitive adaptation have received limited attention (Hickson, 1980). Early research suggests that exercise induces specific adaptations in cognitive functions (Guadagni et al., 2020; Netz, 2019; Smiley-Oyen et al., 2008; Tsai et al., 2019; Voelcker-Rehage & Niemann, 2013), while recent systematic reviews indicate that exercise-related cognitive benefits may be more extensive than previously understood (Ludyga et al., 2020). AET enhances brain health by promoting the growth of new neurons in the hippocampus and blood vessel formation in the cortex, driven by increased levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Tsai et al., 2014) and vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) (Guan & Yan, 2022; Voss et al., 2013). In contrast, SRT improves neuromuscular efficiency and activates the prefrontal cortex through mechanisms involving protein synthesis and the release of muscle-derived factors like irisin (Coelho-Junior et al., 2022; Peng et al., 2017). Additionally, AET enhances cerebral blood flow and glucose metabolism, supporting executive functions (Guadagni et al., 2020), while SRT increases levels of cathepsin B, a muscle-derived factor that reduces brain inflammation (Kim et al., 2019). At the macro-mechanism, both AET and SRT induce adaptive changes in brain structure and function, such as increased gray matter volume, improved white matter integrity, and enhanced connectivity between brain regions, all of which contribute to better cognitive performance (Coelho-Junior et al., 2022; Lopez-Ortiz et al., 2021; Peng et al., 2017; Silva et al., 2024).

Although acute CT may lead to competition for metabolic resources, long-term integration of these training modalities likely amplifies neuroprotection by targeting multiple systems—structural (gray matter), functional (brain connectivity), and molecular (BDNF and irisin) (Coffey & Hawley, 2017). This multifaceted approach aligns with the “cognitive reserve” model, which posits that diverse stimuli build resilience against age-related cognitive decline (Stern, 2002). However, potential interference effects between AET and SRT warrant attention. For example, animal studies suggest that SRT, depending on intensity, may reduce some cognitive benefits and neurogenesis triggered by AET (Lan et al., 2018). The distinct biological mechanisms or inflammatory responses associated with SRT might counteract certain cognitive gains from AET. Thus, understanding the balance between synergy and interference in CT is crucial for designing exercise programs that maximise cognitive benefits, both for elite athletes and broader populations in public health.

In this study, we conducted a moderation analysis of the characteristics of CT rather than according to the FITT-VP principles (Zhang et al., 2023). Consequently, the systematic review and meta-analysis included a wide range of studies without restrictions on publication date or age. The study primarily examined four key issues: (1) the impact of CT on overall cognitive function, reflecting cognitive health across the lifespan; (2) the influence of participant characteristics, including variations across different age groups and health statuses; (3) the moderating effects of training variables, such as intervention duration, frequency, session length, exercise order, and CT configurations, to aid in formulating appropriate training prescriptions; and (4) the impact of study design, considering the nature of the control group and differences in cognitive tasks.

Methods

This systematic review and meta-analysis adhered to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines to ensure methodological transparency and rigorous reporting of results (Liberati et al., 2009). The protocol for this study was pre-registered at PROSPERO (ID: CRD42024593636).

Literature search

Seven English databases (PubMed, Web of Science, EMBASE, CINAHL, Scopus, Google Scholar, Cochrane Library) and three Chinese databases (China National Knowledge Infrastructure, Baidu Scholar and WanFang Data) were independently searched from inception to January 2024, and an updated search was performed on August 2024. Additionally, we conducted a search through Google Scholar to identify unpublished literature and papers not indexed in the Science Citation Index. The search employed a strategic combination of Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) and free-text keywords pertinent to cognition, strength / resistance training, aerobic exercises. The details of search strategy are available in Supplementary file S1. To enhance the comprehensiveness of strategy, the reference lists of identified studies were manually reviewed, and citation tracking was conducted using Elicit (Whitfield & Hofmann, 2023).

Study selection

Two independent reviewers (ZMY and FWF) performed a multi-step screening of titles, abstracts, and full texts. Discrepancies were resolved through discussion, with a third reviewer consulted if needed. Only studies deemed relevant by both reviewers advanced to full-text review.

Eligibility criteria

The inclusion and exclusion criteria were assessed using the PICOS framework (Participants, Intervention, Comparators, Outcomes, Study Design) (Liberati et al., 2009). Eligibility criteria were detailed in Table 1. To investigate the interference effects of CT, as observed in animal models, this review encompasses a diverse range of cognitive statuses and age to address the limited number of published randomised controlled trials (RCTs). By incorporating a variety of populations and study designs, this review aims to provide the first comprehensive evaluation of the effects of CT on cognitive health in human.

Table 1.

Eligibility criteria for meta-analysis.

Category  Inclusion criteria  Exclusion criteria 
Participants  Human with cognitively impaired and unimpaired. There were no restrictions on baseline cognitive status, allowing for a broad inclusion of participants with varying cognitive abilities  Animal model were excluded from meta-analysis 
Intervention  Combined aerobic and strength training (AET + SRT) programs. Programs needed to be explicitly stated as fully supervised and lasting at least 4 weeks to ensure the isolated effects of exercise could be measured.  Single-mode exercise training interventions 
Comparators  Both active and passive control group  Absence of a control group, or involving medical treatments and placebo 
Outcomes  Global cognitive function and cognitive impairment:Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Modified Mini-Mental State Exam (3MSE), Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT), and MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB), and Severe Impairment Battery – Short Form (SIB-S), Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale – Cognitive Sub-Scale (ADAS-Cog)  Lack of baseline and/or post-intervention data 
Study design  peer-reviewed publication, randomised controlled trials  Non-randomised controlled trials, conference abstracts, clinical protocols, observational or quasi-experimental studies 

Note: AET, aerobic / endurance / cardiovascular / cardiorespiratory exercise training; SRT, strength / resistance exercise training; AD, Alzheimer's disease.

Quality assessment

To assess the certainty of evidence for key outcomes, we applied the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) guidelines (Group, 2004). The GRADE approach evaluates evidence quality based on five domains: risk of bias, inconsistency, indirectness, imprecision, and publication bias. Furthermore, the methodological rigor of the studies included in this analysis was rigorously evaluated using the Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool (robvis), a standardised instrument designed to assess critical methodological aspects, including sequence generation, allocation concealment, participant and personnel blinding, outcome assessor blinding, and data completeness (McGuinness & Higgins, 2020). Each study was systematically categorised according to its risk of bias as low, unclear, or high for each criterion. Discrepancies in the risk of bias assessments between the two reviewers were resolved independently through a consensus process; if necessary, a third reviewer was consulted to ensure objectivity.

Data extraction and synthesis

Data extraction was conducted utilizing a standardised form to systematically collect various characteristics, including bibliographic details (first author and year of publication), intervention variables (i.e., frequency, sessional and weekly duration, intensity, volume, length, same session and additional training between AET and SRT), participant characteristics (i.e., sample size, sex, age, healthy status), control conditions (i.e., health education, and routine care/treatment), outcomes [the statistics at the endpoint of the intervention for estimating effect sizes (ES)], study design, and main findings. Based on prior literature (Norton et al., 2010) and guidelines (Baechle & Earle, 2008; Ratamess, 2021), the exercise intensities of AET and SRT were standardised and categorised as sedentary (S), light (L), moderate (M), vigorous (V), and high (H).

Means and standard deviations (SDs) for cognitive measures at baseline and follow-up were extracted for both intervention and control groups. In instances of incomplete reporting of pertinent statistical data, calculations and synthesis were performed using standard errors, within-group confidence intervals (CI), medians, ranges, and p-values, adhering to the formulas delineated in the Cochrane Handbook (Higgins & Green, 2008). Furthermore, missing or improperly formatted data were obtained by contacting the corresponding authors. If means and SDs for each group were not reported, the authors of the primary studies were approached to request baseline and post-intervention data. When data were presented graphically and no additional information was provided upon request, the data were extracted using GetData Graph Digitizer version 2.26 (Sydney, Australia) (Sun et al., 2020).

In cases where multiple articles derived from the same study reported identical or overlapping outcomes, all were included in the systematic review. Additionally, if a study conducted cognitive assessments both post-intervention and during follow-up, only the post-intervention data were extracted. Given that lower ADAS-Cog scores indicate improved cognitive health, the data were inverted to ensure that higher transformed scores consistently reflected enhanced cognitive function across all included measures.

Data coding

CT prescriptions were coded as categorical variables according to established guidelines (García-Hermoso et al., 2018; Northey et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2023). The classifications for the CT are detailed in Supplementary file S2. The characteristics of CT included training configuration (strength training, aerobic exercise, circle-based, and separation), additional training (beyond SRT and AET: yes or no), concurrent design (same week or same session), session duration (short: 30–45 mins, medium: 60 mins, long: 90 mins, or unclear), intervention length (short: 4–12 weeks, medium: 13–26 weeks, long: >26 weeks). Additionally, the frequency of training sessions per week was categorised as low (1–2), medium (3–4), or high (≥5) for both groups.

Global cognition, an umbrella term that encompasses various cognitive domains, including memory, attention, executive function, and visuospatial abilities (Huang et al., 2022; Zhang et al., 2023), was assessed using a range of neuropsychological tests (MMSE, 3MSE, ADAS-Cog, MoCA), categorised according to cognitive moderators.

Regarding age moderators, participant age was classified into two categories based on the reports from each included study: young and middle adulthood (18–65 years) and older adults (>65 years).

Finally, recognizing that the characteristics of the control group may influence the effects of exercise interventions on cognitive health, the control conditions were classified into six categories based on the included studies: health education, no exercise, other exercises, routine activity, routine treatment / care, and social activity (Northey et al., 2018).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted with Stata version 18 and R version 4.2.2 software, employing the metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010). Sandardised mean differences (SMDs) were calculated based on the MD from baseline to follow-up and pooled standard deviations. An inverse variance-weighted random-effects model was applied to the overall ES and 95% CI. A positive ES indicated a greater cognitive benefit in the exercise group compared to the control group, while a negative ES suggested superior cognitive improvement in the control group. Heterogeneity (τ²) was estimated using the restricted maximum likelihood estimator (REML) (Viechtbauer, 2005). To further evaluate heterogeneity, the Q test (Cochran, 1954) and the I² statistic (Higgins & Thompson, 2002) were calculated, with I² values of 20%, 50%, and 75% indicating low, moderate, and high heterogeneity, respectively (Higgins & Green, 2008). If any degree of heterogeneity is detected (i.e., τ² > 0, irrespective of the Q-test results), a prediction interval for the true outcomes is also provided. Studentized residuals and Cook's distances were utilized to assess whether any studies were outliers or influential within the context of the model.

Meta-regression analyses were conducted to investigate potential sources of heterogeneity. Each moderator was incorporated into a random-effects univariate meta-regression analysis utilizing maximum likelihood estimation (Hedges & Olkin, 2014). Subgroup analyses were performed based on single-group sample size (n<50 vs. n≥50) and health status of participants (healthy populations vs. clinical populations), which were conducted to examine potential effect modifiers, including sex, age, intervention characteristics, duration, and frequency. Random-effects models utilizing REML were employed to calculate the ΔES and 95% CI for moderator variables (Gordon et al., 2018). Funnel plots were visually inspected for asymmetry (Duval & Tweedie, 2000). Additionally, Begg rank correlation tests and Egger's test values, along with 95% CI, were calculated using the standard error of the observed outcomes as predictors to statistically assess publication bias (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994; Egger et al., 1997). Small-sample bias was deemed present when the funnel plot exhibited asymmetry and the intercept of Egger's test was significantly different from zero (p < 0.10).

ResultsStudy identification

The initial search strategy yielded a total of 14,643 records obtained from various electronic databases. After the automatic removal of duplicates and irrelevant records, 375 potential studies remained for further screening. The eligibility was thoroughly assessed to eliminate non-relevant studies, leaving 37 studies that met the inclusion criteria. Two studies (Leach et al., 2016; Marzolini et al., 2013) were conducted by the single-group pre-post study design. Overall, 35 studies (Ansai & Rebelatto, 2015; Arrieta et al., 2020; Barnes et al., 2013; Bell et al., 2019; Bossers et al., 2014; Bossers et al., 2015; Callisaya et al., 2017; Cancela Carral & Ayán Pérez, 2008; Carta et al., 2021; da Silveira Langoni et al., 2019; de Oliveira Silva et al., 2019; de Souto Barreto et al., 2017; Espeland et al., 2017; Fonte et al., 2019; Ghodrati et al., 2023; Henskens et al., 2018; Langlois et al., 2013; Lautenschlager et al., 2008; Lu, 2016; Martínez-Velilla et al., 2021; Munguía-Izquierdo & Legaz-Arrese, 2008; Napoli et al., 2014; Nishiguchi et al., 2015; Nuechterlein et al., 2018; Okumiya et al., 1996; Romberg et al., 2005; Jonatan R Ruiz et al., 2015; Shimada et al., 2018; Sink et al., 2015; Suzuki et al., 2013; Tarazona-Santabalbina et al., 2016; Thaiyanto et al., 2021; Timmons et al., 2018; Vreugdenhil et al., 2012; Williamson et al., 2009) ultimately fulfilled the inclusion criteria. The PRISMA study selection process is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) flow diagram of each process of the study selection.

(0.56MB).
Characteristics of included studies

The systematic review and meta-analysis included thirty-five RCTs involving 5734 participants aged 22.4 to 92.1 years, published from 1996 to 2022. The sample comprised 2029 males and 3705 females, with a mean age of 72.9 years. The studies were conducted globally, with four (de Oliveira Silva et al., 2019; Ghodrati et al., 2023; Lu, 2016; Thaiyanto et al., 2021) originating from developing countries. Only one study (Lu, 2016) was published in Chinese, whereas the remaining studies were published in English. Participants represented various age groups, including adolescents (Costigan et al., 2016), young adults (Nuechterlein et al., 2023), middle-aged individuals (Ghodrati et al., 2023; Leach et al., 2016; Marzolini et al., 2013; Munguía-Izquierdo & Legaz-Arrese, 2008; Romberg et al., 2005), and the elderly (Ansai & Rebelatto, 2015; Arrieta et al., 2020; Barnes et al., 2013; Bell et al., 2019; Bossers et al., 2014; Bossers et al., 2015; Callisaya et al., 2017; Cancela Carral & Ayán Pérez, 2008; Carta et al., 2021; da Silveira Langoni et al., 2019; de Oliveira Silva et al., 2019; de Souto Barreto et al., 2017; Espeland et al., 2017; Fonte et al., 2019; Henskens et al., 2018; Langlois et al., 2013; Lautenschlager et al., 2008; Lu, 2016; Martínez-Velilla et al., 2021; Napoli et al., 2014; Nishiguchi et al., 2015; Okumiya et al., 1996; Romberg et al., 2005; Jonatan R Ruiz et al., 2015; Shimada et al., 2018; Sink et al., 2015; Suzuki et al., 2013; Tarazona-Santabalbina et al., 2016; Thaiyanto et al., 2021; Timmons et al., 2018; Vreugdenhil et al., 2012; Williamson et al., 2009), with the majority (5382 individuals) classified as older adults. Notably, there is a paucity of research concerning the cognitive health of children and adolescents in the concurrent exercise interventions. Participants were categorised into healthy and clinical populations, with the latter further subdivided into individuals with physiological disorders, mental disorders, cognitive impairments, and age-related cognitive decline.

AET exhibited considerable variability, with regular walking (Arrieta et al., 2020; Bossers et al., 2014; Bossers et al., 2015; Callisaya et al., 2017; da Silveira Langoni et al., 2019; de Oliveira Silva et al., 2019; de Souto Barreto et al., 2017; Espeland et al., 2017; Fonte et al., 2019; Henskens et al., 2018; Lautenschlager et al., 2008; Martínez-Velilla et al., 2021; Marzolini et al., 2013; Nishiguchi et al., 2015; Okumiya et al., 1996; Shimada et al., 2018; Sink et al., 2015; Suzuki et al., 2013; Tarazona-Santabalbina et al., 2016; Vreugdenhil et al., 2012; Williamson et al., 2009) identified as the most common activity, supplemented by running (Costigan et al., 2016; de Oliveira Silva et al., 2019), cycling (Ansai & Rebelatto, 2015; Callisaya et al., 2017; Fonte et al., 2019; Ghodrati et al., 2023; Langlois et al., 2013; Marzolini et al., 2013; Jonatan R Ruiz et al., 2015; Timmons et al., 2018), stepping (Shimada et al., 2018; Tarazona-Santabalbina et al., 2016), swimming, rowing (Callisaya et al., 2017), aerobics (Ghodrati et al., 2023; Lu, 2016; Tarazona-Santabalbina et al., 2016; Thaiyanto et al., 2021), dance-based aerobics (Barnes et al., 2013), and jumping jacks (Costigan et al., 2016). Additionally, SRT primarily utilized four modalities: body-weight (Ansai & Rebelatto, 2015; Bossers et al., 2014; Bossers et al., 2015; Callisaya et al., 2017; Costigan et al., 2016; da Silveira Langoni et al., 2019; Martínez-Velilla et al., 2021; Marzolini et al., 2013; Okumiya et al., 1996; Shimada et al., 2018; Thaiyanto et al., 2021), free weights, weight-lifting machines (Arrieta et al., 2020; Bell et al., 2019; Cancela Carral & Ayán Pérez, 2008; de Oliveira Silva et al., 2019; Fonte et al., 2019; Ghodrati et al., 2023; Henskens et al., 2018; Lu, 2016; Napoli et al., 2014; Jonatan R Ruiz et al., 2015; Timmons et al., 2018), and resistance bands (da Silveira Langoni et al., 2019; Marzolini et al., 2013; Romberg et al., 2005; Jonatan R Ruiz et al., 2015; Tarazona-Santabalbina et al., 2016; Thaiyanto et al., 2021). Four studies (Bell et al., 2019; Costigan et al., 2016; Nuechterlein et al., 2018; Timmons et al., 2018) employed circuit training based on HIIT or MIIT, indicating mixed-order cross-training sessions for AET and SRT. The intensity of AET was adjusted using maximum heart rate (%HRmax), heart rate reserve (%HRR), or maximal oxygen uptake (VO2 max), while the intensity of SRT was monitored through the rate of perceived exertion (RPE scales 6–20) (Borg, 1998), percentage of one-repetition maximum (%1RM), or the amount of weight lifted, to assess absolute or relative intensity. Nine studies (Henskens et al., 2018; Lautenschlager et al., 2008; Leach et al., 2016; Lu, 2016; Martínez-Velilla et al., 2021; Nishiguchi et al., 2015; Okumiya et al., 1996; Romberg et al., 2005; Vreugdenhil et al., 2012) did not report load intensity details for both exercise types. These diverse intervention characteristics facilitated a comprehensive analysis of the effects of CT on brain and cognitive health. Exercise frequency ranged from one to eight sessions per week, with session durations spanning 30 to 120 minutes. The majority of interventions lasted less than 16 weeks, with a few extending to 8, 12, or 14 weeks.

In terms of comparators, five studies (Ansai & Rebelatto, 2015; Bossers et al., 2014; Bossers et al., 2015; Costigan et al., 2016; Timmons et al., 2018) compared CT with AET alone (Bossers et al., 2014; Bossers et al., 2015; Costigan et al., 2016; Timmons et al., 2018) or SRT alone (Ansai & Rebelatto, 2015; Costigan et al., 2016; Timmons et al., 2018). Control group included routine activities, no exercise, other exercises, routine care/treatment, social activity, cognitive training, or health education. One study (Nuechterlein et al., 2018) compared CT plus cognitive training to cognitive training alone, while another (Bell et al., 2019) compared CT with nutritional supplementation to CT alone. Furthermore, two studies (Leach et al., 2016; Marzolini et al., 2013) employed a single-group pre-post test design.

The included studies evaluated a range of outcomes related to brain and cognitive health associated with CT. All studies examined the effects of CT on global cognitive function, with seven (Arrieta et al., 2020; Callisaya et al., 2017; Ghodrati et al., 2023; Nishiguchi et al., 2015; Nuechterlein et al., 2018; Jonatan R Ruiz et al., 2015; Suzuki et al., 2013) providing data on structural and biological markers pertinent to brain health. A diverse array of cognitive assessment tools was employed, including the MMSE, 3MSE, MoCA, ACE-R, PASAT, MCCB, SIB-S, and ADAS-Cog. Various tasks were frequently utilized to assess the same cognitive domain within or across studies. A comprehensive description and findings of the study characteristics are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Characteristics of the included studies.

Reference (Country / region)YearPopulation characteristics (age, n, gender, status)DesignGroupsInterventionSeparate sessionsFrequency and durationCognitive taskKey findings
SRT*  AET* 
Munguía-Izquierdo and Legaz-Arrese (2008)(Spain)  2008  Age:47.7±8.3126 F66.7% FM + 33.3% Healthy  RCT  EG (n=29) : concurrent aerobic and resistance trainingCG (n=24) : routine activity  Type *: aquatic resistance contractionIntensity: L-MVolume:sets × 1–3reps × 8–15Time: 10–20 min  Type: aquatic exercisesIntensity ↑: L-V50–80% HRmaxTime: 20–30 min  No  60 min × 3 / wk, 16 wks  PASAT  ↑ CognitionMD=6.7 (3.1, 10.3), p=0.004 
Bell et al. (2019)(Canada)  2019  Age: 73 ± 649 MHealthy  RCT  EG1 (n=18) : concurrent aerobic and resistance exercise + supplementEG2 (n=20) : Concurrent exercise  Type*: weight-lifting machines and free weightIntensity: H80% 1RMVolume:sets 3reps 6–8Intervals 60-s  Type: ridingIntensity: H10 × 60 s HIIT(1: 1)∼90% HRmaxTime: 20-min  Yes  2 SRT + 1 HIIT / wks, 12 wks  MoCA, RAVLT, RT-S, Go-No-Go task  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑MOCA (p = 0.013)

  • ↑RAVLT (p = 0.047) and↓reaction time (p = 0.002)

 
Suzuki et al. (2013) (Japan)  2013  Age:75.3±6.849F/51M50% Healthy + 50% aMCI  RCT  EG (n=50) : concurrent aerobic and resistance exerciseCG (n=50) : Health education  Type*: nrIntensity: nrVolume: 20-min  Type: walkingIntensity: M∼60% HRmaxTime: 20–30 min  No  90 min × 2 / wk, 6 or 12 months  MMSE, WMS-LM II, DSC, VFT-L+C, SCWT, ADAS-cog, and brain atrophy measurements  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MMSE: MD = 0.3 (-0.5, 0.9), p = 0.04

  • ↑immediate memory: MD = 2.8 (1.4, 4.2), p < 0.01

  • ↑ delayed memory: MD=3.4 (2.0, 4.8), p < 0.01

  • ↓brain atrophy: MD = 0.1 (-0.4, 0.7), p < 0.05

 
Leach et al. (2016)a(Canada)  2016  Age:50.5±8.763 FBreast cancer  Pre-post study  EG (n=63) : BEAUTY program, including aerobic and strength exercise  Type: nrIntensity: nrVolume: nr  Type: nrIntensity: nrTime: nr  No  SRT 1 / wk, AET 2 / wk, 24 wks  FACT-cog  ? FACT-cog: MD= 0.9, p > 0.05 
Lautenschlager et al. (2008)(Australia)  2008  Age:68.7±8.686F/84MAD  RCT  EG (n=85) : home-based concurrent aerobic and resistance exerciseCG (n=85) : routine care  Type: nrIntensity: LVolume: nr  Type: walkingIntensity: MTime: nr  nr  50 min × 3 / wk, 18 months  ADAS-Cog, WLDR  ↑ Cognition
  • ↓ADAS-Cog: MD= -0.73 (-1.27, 0.03), p = 0.04

 
Ansai and Rebelatto (2015)(Brazil)  2014  Age: 82.4±2.447F/22MHealthy  RCT  EG1(n=23): aerobic and strength exerciseEG2(n=23): resistance trainingCG (n=23) : routine activity  Type: free weight or body-weightIntensity ↑: V-HRPE 14–17Volume ↑:reps (up to 15)series (up to 3)  Type *: cycle ergometerIntensity: V60–85% HRRTime: ∼13-min  No  60 min × 3 / wk, 16 wks  MoCA, CDT, VFT  ? MoCA: p > 0.05 
Carta et al. (2021)(Italy)  2021  Age:72.2±4.763F/42MHealthy  RCT  EG (n=52) : integrated aerobic and strength exercisesCG (n=53) : cultural and educational activities  Type:life movementsIntensity: V60–84% HRRVolume: 45-min  Type: nrIntensity: V60–84% HRRTime: 45-min  No  60 min × 3 /week,12 wks  ACE-R  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ ACE-R: MD = 2.4, p = 0.040

  • ↑ Memory: MD = 1.4, p = 0.022

  • ↑ visual-spatial skills: MD = 0.6, p = 0.046

 
Nuechterlein et al. (2018)(USA)  2023  Age:22.4±3.914F/33MSchizophrenia  RCT  EG (n=24) : cognitive training + concurrent circuit trainingCG (n=23) : cognitive training  Type:MIIT circuitIntensity: nrVolume: 15-min  Type:MIIT circuitIntensity: V60–80% HRRTime: 30/45 min  No  60 min × 4 /week,24 wks  MCCB, BDNF, Global Functioning Scale  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MCCB: MD = 6.5, p = 0.012

  • ↑ work/school functioning: MD = 1.8, p < 0.001

  • ? BDNF: MD = 3614, p > 0.05

 
Nishiguchi et al. (2015)(Japan)  2015  Age:73.0±4.822F/26MHealthy  RCT  EG (n=24) : multimodal exerciseCG (n=24) : no exercise  Type*: nrIntensity ↑: nrVolume: nr  Type: walkingIntensity: LTime↑: increase daily steps by 15% each month  No  90 min × 1 / wk,12 wks  MMSE, WMS-LM I/II, TMT, and fMRI  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MMSE: MD = 0.8, p = 0.009

  • ↑ memory: MD = 4.7 - 5.2, p = 0.008 - 0.009

  • ↑ executive function: MD = 13.2, p = 0.018

  • ↓ brain activation efficiency: p < 0.001

 
Cancela Carral and Ayán Pérez (2008)(Spain)  2007  Age: 68.4±3.462 FHealthy  RCT  EG (n=27) : water calisthenic exercises combined with strength trainingCG (n=29) : calisthenic exercises alone  Type: weight-lifting machinesIntensity: M 75%1RMVolume:sets × 3reps × 10  Type: swimmingIntensity: HTime: 45-min  No  AET 45 min × 5 + RT × 3 / week,5 wks  MMSE  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MMSE: EG (MD = 2.61, p = 0.03) < CG (MD = 5, p = 0.02)

 
Bossers et al. (2014) (the Netherlands)  2014  Age: 85.2±6.425F/8MDementia  RCT  EG (n=17) : combined aerobic and strength exerciseCG (n=16) : social activity  Type: free weight, body-weightIntensity: M-V RPE 12–15Weight: body-weight →1.5 kgVolume:sets × 3reps × 8–30  Type*: outdoor walkingIntensity: M-V RPE 12–15Time: 30-min  Yes  30 min × 5 (2 RT+3 AET) /week,6 wks  MMSE, FRT, PRT, VMS-F + B, DST-B, VFT, PCT, Stroop task  ? Cognition
  • ? MMSE: ES = 0.13, p = 0.657

  • ? FRT: ES = 0.13, p = 0.114

  • ↑ VMS-F: ES = 0.68, P = 0.022

  • ? VMS-B: ES = 0.03, p = 0.502

  • ? DST-B: ES = 0.21, p = 0.576

  • ? Stroop task: ES = 0.23, p = 0.721

 
Bossers et al. (2015)(the Netherlands)  2015  Age: 85.5±5.182F/27MDementia  RCT  EG1 (n= 37) : concurrent aerobic and resistance exercise programEG2 (n= 36) : aerobic exercise aloneCG (n=36) : social activity  Type: free weightIntensity ↑:body-weight→1.5kgVolume↑:sets 3 × 2reps 8→12  Type*: walkingIntensity: M-Hdistances-basedTime: 30-min  Yes  30 min × 4 (2 ST+2 AET) / wk, 9 wks  MMSE, Visual memory, Verbal memory  ↑Cognition and ↓ cognitive decline EG > CG
  • ↑ MMSE: ES = 0.48, p < 0.001

  • ↑ verbal memory: ES = 0.37, p = 0.032

  • ↑ visual memory: ES = 0.46, p = 0.008

  • ↑ executive function: ES = 0.37, p < 0.001

 
Langlois et al. (2013) (Canada)  2012  Age:72.4±5.756F/16M52.7% Healthy + 47.3% Frail  RCT  EG (n=36) : physical exercise training programCG (n=36) : routine activity  Type: nr.Intensity: M-HVolume: 10 min  Type*: treadmills, recumbent riding, and ellipticalIntensity ↑: M→HTime: 10–30 min  No  60 min × 3 /wk, 12 wks  DSC, VFT, VS, RAVLT  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ executive functions (p = 0.039)

  • ↑ processing speed (p = 0.014)

↑ working memory: (p = 0.035) 
Williamson et al. (2009)(USA)  2009  Age: 77.4±4.372F/30MHealthy  RCT  EG(n=50): physical activity, including aerobic, strength exercisesCG(n=52): health education  Type: nr.Intensity: MVolume: nr.  Type: walkingIntensity: M60%-70% HRmaxTime: 40–60 min  nr  ≥40–60 min × 3/2+3 /wk, 48 wks  3MSE, DSST, RAVLT, and modified Stroop test  ? No improvement
  • ↑ DSST: MD = 0.56, p < 0.05

  • ? RAVLT (p > 0.05)

  • ? 3MSE (p > 0.05)

? modified Stroop Test (p > 0.05) 
Sink et al. (2015)(USA)  2015  Age:78.9±5.2999F/477M189 MCI or dementia  RCT  EG (n=735) : physical activity interventionCG (n=741) : successful aging health education  Type:free weightIntensity: MVolume: 10-min  Type*: walkingIntensity: MTime: 30-min  No  ∼60 min × 2+3–4 /wk, 96 wks  DSC, HVLT-R, 3MSE  ? No improvement
  • ? DSC (p = 0.97)

? HVLT-R (p = 0.84) 
Thaiyanto et al. (2021)(Thailand)  2020  Age:67.8±2.540 FMCI + AD  RCT  EG (n=20) : multicomponent exercise including aerobic and resistance exercisesCG (n=20) : health education  Type*: resistance band or body-weightIntensity ↑: M increase by repetitions or loadRPE ≤13–14Volume↑:15-min  Type: aerobics Intensity ↑: M-VRPE 13–14Time: 15-min  No  60 min × 3 /wk, 12 wks  ADAS-Cog, TMT, Stroop test  ↑ Attention and executive functionTMT-A (p < 0.05) 
de Oliveira Silva et al. (2019)(Brazil)  2019  Age:77.2±7.027F/19MMCI + AD  RCT  EG (n=24) : multimodal exercise program (including aerobic and resistance)CG (n=14) : no exercise  Type: weight-lifting machinesIntensity: MVolume:sets 3reps 8–12  Type*: treadmill running or walkingIntensity: V70% VO2max / 80% HRmaxTime: 30-min  No  60 min × 2 /wk, 12 wks  MMSE, CDT, VFT, ST3  ↑ verbal fluency↑ VFT (p = 0.05) 
Napoli et al. (2014)(USA)  2014  Age:69 ± 472F/35MHealthy  RCT  EG (n=54) : exercise (+ diet) programCG (n=53) : health education (+diet)  Type: weight-lifting machinesIntensity ↑:M→H∼65 → ∼80% 1RMVolume: 30 minsets 1–3reps 6–12  Type*: nrIntensity ↑: V-H∼65% → 70–85% HRpeakTime: 30-min  No  90 min × 3 /wk, 52 wks  3MSE, WLF, TMT A+B  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ 3MSE: MD = 1.7, p = 0.0001–0.04

  • ↑ WLF: MD = 4.1, p = 0.001

  • ↑ TMT-A: MD = 211.8, p = 0.001

↑ TMT-B: MD = 221.8, p = 0.001 
Shimada et al. (2018)(Japan)  2018  Age:71.6±5.0154F/154MMCI  RCT  EG (n=154) : combined activity program, including aerobic exercise, muscle strength trainingCG (n=154) : health education  Type*: free weight or body-weightIntensity: nrVolume: 20-min  Type: stepping and walkingIntensity: M-V60–80% HRmaxTime: 25-min  No  90 min × 1 /wk, 40 wks  MMSE, WMS-LM II, RAVLT, VFT-letter, VFT-category, TMT  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MMSE (p = 0.012)

  • ↑ WMS-LM II (p = 0.004)

  • ? RAVLT (p = 0.352)

  • ↑ VFT-letter (p < 0.001)

↑ VFT-category (p = 0.002) 
Timmons et al. (2018)(Ireland)  2018  Age: 69.3±3.539F/45MHealthy  RCT  EG1 (n=21) : aerobic exercise aloneEG2 (n=21) : resistance exercise aloneEG3 (n=21) : concurrent exerciseCG (n=21) : no exercise  Type*: Circuit-basedweight-lifting machines and free weightIntensity: M∼60% 1RMVolume:reps for 1 minrounds × 2exercise × 6recovery × 30s  Type:MIIT-based cycle ergometerIntensity: V∼80% HRmaxTime: 12-minsets × 1.5recovery for 1-min  No  40 min × 3 / wk, 12 wks  MoCA  ↑ Cognition↑ MoCA (p < 0.001) 
Henskens et al. (2018)(the Netherlands)  2018  Age:85.7±5.667F/20MDementia  RCT  EG (n=22) : concurrent exercise trainingCG (n=22) : social activity  Type: weight-lifting machines or free weightIntensity ↑: nr weight ↑Volume:sets × 3reps 8→15  Type: outdoor walkingIntensity ↑: nr optimal walking speedTime:30–45 min  Yes  30–45 min × 3 /wk, 6 months  MMSE, SIB-S, executive function: DST-B, CFT, Go-No-Go test, 
  • ? MMSE (p = 0.83)

  • ? SIB-S (p = 0.85)

  • ? DST-F (p = 0.13)

  • DST-B (p = 0.88)

  • ↑ EF (p = 0.007)

  • ↑ CFT (p = 0.004)

↑ Go-No-Go test (p = 0.04) 
Espeland et al. (2017)(USA)  2016  Age:70–89899F/477MDiabetes  RCT  EG (n=735) : aerobic, resistance training, and flexibility exercisesCG (n=741) : health education  Type: free weightIntensity: VRPE 15–16Volume: 10-min  Type*: walkingIntensity: MRPE 13Time: 30-min  No  ∼60 min × 2 CB + 3–4 HB /wk, 26 wks  3MSE, DSC, HVLT-R  ↑ Global cognition
  • ↑ 3MSE (p = 0.02)

  • ? DSC (p = 0.87)

  • ↑ HVLT-R (p = 0.005)

? EF (p = 0.70) 
Costigan et al. (2016)(Australia)  2016  Age:15.8 ± 0.620F/45MHealthy  RCT  EG1 (n=21) : aerobic exercise programEG2 (n=22) : concurrent resistance and aerobic programCG (n=22) : physical education and usual lunchtime activities  Type: HIIT-based body-weightIntensity: V≥85% HRmaxVolume:reps for 30-s,work-to-rest ratio: 30:30  Type: HIIT shuttle runs, jumping jacks, and skippingIntensity: V≥85%HRmaxTime: 8–10 min,work-to-rest ratio: 30:30  No  8–10 min × 3/5 (3–4 ST+1 AET) / wk, 8 wks  TMT  ↑ Executive function (p < 0.05) 
Romberg et al. (2005)(Finland)  2005  Age: 43.9±6.761F/34MMS  RCT  EG (n=47) : concurrent resistance and aerobic trainingCG (n=48) : no intervention  Type: resistance bandIntensity: nrVolume: nr  Type: nr.Intensity: nrTime: nr.  Yes  4–6 (3–5 ST+1AET) /wk, 26 wks  PASAT  ༟No improvement (p = 0.379) 
Vreugdenhil et al. (2012)(Australia)  2012  Age:74.1(51–89)24F/16MAD  RCT  EG (n=20) : home-based concurrent exerciseCG (n=20) : routine treatment  Type: nrIntensity: nrVolume: nr  Type: walkingIntensity: nr.Time: ≥30 min  No  30 min × daily, 4 months  ADAS-Cog, MMSE  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MMSE: MD = 2.6, p < 0.001

↓ADAS-Cog: MD = -7.1, p <0.001 
Arrieta et al. (2020)(Spain)  2020  Age:84.9±6.879F/33MHealthy  RCT  EG (n=43) :multicomponent exercise program, including strength and walkingCG (n=45) : routine activity  Type*: free weight or weight-lifting machinesIntensity ↑: L-M40–70%1RM →65–70%1RMVolume:sets 1–2reps 7–12exercise × 3–5  Type: walkingIntensity ↑: MCR10 scale 5–6Time ↑: 5→22 min  No  45 min × 2 / wk, 6 months  MoCA, RAVLT, TMT-A, WAIS-IV, VFT, SFT  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MoCA (p = 0.003)

  • ↑ Symbol search (p = 0.049)

  • ? TMT-A (p = 0.836)

  • ↑ WAIS-IV (p < 0.05)

  • ↑ VFT (p < 0.05)

  • ↑ SFT (p < 0.05)

  • ? serum BDNF levels (p > 0.05)

 
de Souto Barreto et al. (2017)(France)  2017  Age:87.6±5.577F/14MDementia  C-RCT  EG (n=47) : group-based exerciseCG (n=50) : social activity  Type*:free weightIntensity ↑: MVolume↑:10–15 min  Type: walkingIntensity ↑: MTime:20-min  No  60 min × 2 / wk, 24 wks  MMSE  ? No improvementMMSE (p = 0.43) 
Jonatan R Ruiz et al. (2015)(Spain)  2015  Age: 92.1±2.132F/8M  RCT  EG (n=20) : aerobic and resistance exercisesCG (n=20) : routine care  Type: resistance band or weight-lifting machinesIntensity: L→M↑Volume:sets 2–3reps 8–10interval 1–2 min  Type*: cycle-ergometerIntensity: L-MRPE 10–12Time: 10–15 min  No  40–45 min × 3 / wk, 8 wks  MMSE,ACE, BDNF, EGF, TNF-α  ? No improvement
  • MMSE (p = 0.97)

 
Ghodrati et al. (2023)(Iran)  2022  Age:57.8±1.521 FDiabetes  RCT  EG (n=12) : concurrent trainingCG (n=9) : no exercise  Type: weight-lifting machinesIntensity ↑:M→H65%→85% 1RMVolume↑:sets 3reps 6–12intervals for 1-minTime: 30-min  Type*: aerobics, elliptical trainer and stationary bicycleIntensity ↑: M-V55%→75%HRRSets 4Time: 20-min  No  ∼65 min × 3 / wk, 12 wks  MoCA, DSST, DST-F  ? Cognition
  • MoCA (p = 0.05)

  • DSST (p = 0.33)

  • DST-F (p = 0.07)

  • ༟BDNF or irisin levels (p > 0.05)

 
Barnes et al. (2013)(USA)  2013  Age:73.4±5.979F/47MHealthy  RCT  EG (n=31) : concurrent aerobic and strength exerciseCG (n=32) : other exercises  Type: nrIntensity: nrVolume: 10-min  Type*:aerobic danceIntensity: V60–75% HRpeakTime: 30-min  No  60 min × 3 / wk, 12 wks  3MSE, RAVLT, DSST, TMT-A+B, EFT, UFOV,  ? Cognition
  • ↑3MSE: MD = 0.16 (0.05, 0.26), p < 0.001

  • RAVLT: p= 0.38, p = 0.93

  • DSST: p = 0.71

  • TMT-A: p = 0.24

  • TMT-B: p = 0.31

  • EFT: p = 0.51, p = 0.60

  • UFOV

  • processing speed (p = 0 0.65)

  • divided attention (p = 0.05)

↑selective attention (p = 0.02) 
Martínez-Velilla et al. (2021)(Spain)  2021  Age:86.5±553F/50MDiabetes  RCT  EG (n=54) : multicomponent exerciseCG (n=49) : usual care  Type: body-weight or free weightIntensity ↑: nrVolume:sets 3reps 12  Type: walkingIntensity: LTime: 20-min  No  40 min × 5–7 / wk, 12 wks  MMSE  ↑ CognitionMMSE: MD = 1.7, p <0.001 
Callisaya et al. (2017)(Australia)  2017  Age:66.2±4.924F/26M Diabetes  RCT  EG (n=26) : concurrent aerobic and resistance trainingCG (n=24) : other exercises  Type*: body-weight, weight-lifting machine or free weightsIntensity ↑:V-HRPE 14–17Volume: 30-minsets 3reps 8–12  Type: stationary cycling, cross trainer, rower or treadmill walkingIntensity ↑:M→V→HRPE 12→16Time: 30-min  No  60 min × 3 / wk, 6 months  TMT, DSC, WMS-LM Ⅲ, COWAT, HVLT-R, RCF, DST-F+B, and Stroop test  ? Cognition
  • MMSE: MD = 0.14

  • EF: MD = -3.8 (-7.1, -0.5)

  • processing speed: MD = -8.7 (-17.9, 0.5)

  • DSC: MD = +2.2 (0.2, 4.2)

  • HVLT-R: MD1 = 0.7 (-0.9, 2.2), MD2=0.1 (-0.7, 0.9)

  • COWAT: MD1 = 3.1 (0.4, 5.7), MD2 = 0.9 (-2.6, 0.8)

  • ↑ white matter integrity, hippocampal and total brain volumes

 
da Silveira Langoni et al. (2019)(Brazil)  2019  Age:72.3±7.940F/12MMCI  RCT  EG (n=26) : group-based aerobic and strength training programCG (n=26) : no exercise  Type: free weight, resistance band, or body-weightIntensity ↑: nrVolume ↑:30-minsets 2reps 10–15  Type: walkingIntensity:60–75% HRmaxTime ↑:20-min → 25-min → 30-min  No  ∼60 min × 4 / wk, 6 months  MMSE  ↑ Global cognition↑ MMSE: MD = 3.1 ± 2.4, p < 0.001 
Marzolini et al. (2013)(Canada)  2013  Age:63.6±13.511F/30MPost-stroke  Pre-post study  EG (n=41) : concurrent aerobic and resistance exercise  Type: free weight, resistance band, or body-weightIntensity: M50–60% 1RMRPE 13–14Volume:Sets based on RPEreps 10–15  Type *: walking, cyclingIntensity: L-V40–70% VO2peakRPE 11–16Time: 20–60 min  Unclear  90 min × 1+5 (home-based:4 AET+1–2 ST) / wk, 6 months  MoCA  ↑ Cognition
  • ↑ MoCA: MD = 1.5, p < 0.001

  • ↑ attention: MD = 0.5, p =0.03

↑ executive function: MD = 0.5, p = 0.002 
Tarazona-Santabalbina et al. (2016)(Spain)  2016  Age:80.0±3.754F/46MHealthy  RCT  EG (n=51) : multicomponent exercise programCG (n=49) : routine care  Type: resistance bandIntensity ↑:L→M25→75%1RMVolume:sets 1–3reps 8–30  Type*: walking, aerobics, stepIntensity ↑: L-M40–65% HRmaxTime: 40-min  Yes  40 min × 5 (2ST + 3AET) / wk, 14 wks  MMSE  ↑ Global cognitionMMSE: MD = 2.4, p = 0.025 
Fonte et al. (2019)(Italy)  2019  Age:78±655 F/32M31% MCI +69% AD  RCT  EG (n=27) : endurance and resistance trainingCG (n=30) : no intervention  Type: isotonic ergometersweight-lifting machinesIntensity ↑: H85%1RMVolume ↑:sets 3reps 12  Type*: cycling, treadmill walking, arm crankingIntensity ↑: M-V70% HRmaxTime: 45-min  No  90 min × 3 / wk, 24 wks  MMSE, FAB, TMT-A+B, RBMT, DCT , ADAS-Cog  ↑ Global cognition and ↓cognitive decline
  • MMSE: MD = 0%

  • TMT-A: MD = -12.4%

  • TMT-B: MD = -7.8%

  • RBMT: MD = +6.9%

 
Okumiya et al. (1996)(Japan)  1996  Age: 78.8±4.624F/18MHealthy  RCT  EG (n=21) : multicomponent exerciseCG (n=21): no exercise  Type: body-weightIntensity: LVolume: nr  Type*: walkingIntensity: LTime: 50-min  No  60 min × 2 / wk, 24 wks  MMSE, HDS-R, VCP-test  ? No improvement
  • MMSE (p > 0.05)

  • HDS-R (p > 0.05)

VCP-test (p > 0.05) 
Lu (2016)(China)  2016  Age:74.5±4.6 70F/78MMCI  RCT  EG (n=74) : concurrent resistance and aerobic trainingCG (n=74): rehabilitation training and routine health education  Type: free weight or weight-lifting machinesIntensity: M-HVolume: nr  Type: aerobicsIntensity: M-HTime: 40-min  No  60 min × 5 / wk, 12 months  MoCA  ? No improvement
  • MoCA (p > 0.05)

 

RAVLT: the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, PASAT: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, MMSE: Mini-mental state examination, 3MSE: modified Mini-mental state exam, MoCA: Montréal Cognitive Assessment, SCWT: Stroop color word test, WLDR: word list delayed recall, CDT: clock drawing test, VFT: verbal fluency test, VFT-L+C: letter and categorical verbal fluency test, CFT: category fluency test, SFT: semantic fluency tests, WLF: word list fluency test, RT-S: simple reaction time task, RT-C: choice reaction time task, MCCB: MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, fMRI: functional magnetic resonance imaging, FRT: faces recognition test, PRT: pictures recognition test, DSC: digit symbol coding test, DCT: digit cancellation test, DSST: digit symbol substitution test, DST-F+B: digit span test forward and backward, ADAS-Cog: Alzheimer Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale, FACT-Cog: Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy - Cognitive Function; ACE-R: Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Revised, TMT-A+B: Trail making part A+B, RNG: random number generation task, VMS-F+B: visual memory span forward and backward, PCT: picture completion test, VS: visual-spatial abilities, EFT: Eriksen Flanker test, UFOV: Useful field of view, RCF: Rey complex figure, COWAT: controlled oral word association test, FAB: frontal assessment battery, VCP-test: Visuo-spatial cognitive performance test, HDS-R: Hasegawa Dementia Scale-Revised, RBMT: Rivermead behavioral memory test, HVLT-R: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, SIB-S: severe impairment battery – short form, WMS-LM II: logical memory subtest of the Wechsler memory scale-revised, WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – 3rd Edition, BDNF: brain - derived neural factor, ACE: angiotensin – converting enzyme, EGF: epidermal growth factor, TNF-α: tumor necrosis factor-α;

AET: aerobic / endurance / cardiovascular / cardiorespiratory exercise training, SRT: strength / resistance exercise training, EG: experimental group, CG: control group, wk: week, HIIT: high-intensity interval training, MIIT: moderate-intensity interval training, BEAUTY program: Breast cancer patients Engaging in Activity while Undergoing Treatment, HRmax / HRpeak: maximum heart rate, %HRR: heart rate reserve =HRmax −resting HR, %VO2max: maximal oxygen uptake, MAP: maximal aerobic power, 1RM: one-repetition maximum, Reps: number of repetitions, RPE: Rate of Perceived Exertion, based on Borg RPE scale, including category scale (6–20) and category-ratio scale (0–10); METs: Metabolic Equivalent, 1METs=3.5ml O2/kg/min, S: sedentary, L: light, M: moderate, V: vigorous, H: high, MD: mean difference, ES: effect size, Cohen's d;

FM: Fibromyalgia syndrome, (a)MCI: (amnestic) mild cognitive impairment, AD: Alzheimer's disease, MS: multiple sclerosis;

* indicating that this exercise was performed before the other exercise

↑ indicating a progressive load from baseline to post-intervention

a

indicating study was conducted with pre-post design

Risk of bias

The risk of bias assessment is summarised in Supplementary file S3 and Fig. 2. Most studies exhibited a low risk for random sequence generation and outcome assessment blinding. However, allocation concealment and participant blinding were frequently unclear or high-risk. Incomplete outcome data and selective reporting presented moderate risks, while other sources of bias were minimal.

Fig. 2.

Analysis of the risk of bias indicated by percentages of assessed biases in included studies.

(0.27MB).
Overall analyses and quality of evidence

The forest plot of the effects of CT interventions on global cognition is presented in Fig. 3. A total of k=41 dependent ES were included in the overall analysis. The overall ES revealed by the meta-analysis was significant and positive (SMD = 0.32; 95% CI: 0.17–0.46, p < 0.001). Twelve statistically significant results (p < 0.05) are shown in Supplementary file S4, with nine having p-values below 0.025, all included in the p-curve. The p-curve analysis revealed significant effects with a power estimate of 91%, supporting the research hypothesis. However, substantial heterogeneity was noted (Q40 = 178.6, p < 0.0001, tau² = 0.21, I² = 86.0%) (Higgins et al., 2003).

Fig. 3.

Forest plot of the overall effects of concurrent training on global cognition. CI, confidence interval.

(0.57MB).

The GRADE analysis indicated that the overall quality of evidence was moderate. Key limitations were identified, including variability across studies (inconsistency) and the potential for publication bias. The funnel plot of included studies is displayed in Supplementary file S5. An examination of the studentized residuals revealed that one study (Napoli et al., 2014) had a value larger than ± 3.23 and may be a potential outlier in the context of this model. According to the Cook's distances, two studies (da Silveira Langoni et al., 2019; Napoli et al., 2014) could be considered to be overly influential. Neither the Begg rank correlation nor the Egger's regression test indicated any funnel plot asymmetry (p = 0.76 and p = 0.08, respectively) and potential publication bias. Fig. 4 showed that Galbraith plot uncovered significant heterogeneity in the study results; therefore, moderator analyses, subgroup analysis and meta regression were conducted.

Fig. 4.

Galbraith plot for assessing the heterogeneity in meta-analysis.

(0.14MB).
Moderator analyses

To investigate potential sources of variance in the effects of CT on global cognitive function, moderator analyses were conducted using distinct models. The findings from this analysis are summarised in Table 3. For each moderator analysis, the omnibus test yielded significant results, and the 95% CI for all factors overlapped.

Table 3.

Moderator analysis of effects of concurrent training on cognitive function outcomes.

Moderator  Hedges' g (95%CI)  Q statistic  P value 
Participants moderators
Age, year         
18–65  0.24 (0.00 to 0.48)  Q = 145.76; I2=83.38; p<0.010.099 
>65  31  0.33 (0.14 to 0.51)  0.004 
Health status         
Healthy population  12  0.59 (-0.03 to 1.20)  Q = 151.13; I2=84.57; p<0.010.063 
Clinical population  20  0.28 (0.11 to 0.46)  <0.001 
Both  0.05 (-0.05 to 0.15)  0.33 
Exercise moderators
Same session         
Yes  30  0.38 (0.10 to 0.66)  Q = 158.82; I2=82.23 p<0.010.007 
No  0.39 (0.09 to 0.68)  0.011 
Additional exercises         
Yes  18  0.26 (0.10 to 0.42)  Q = 182.96; I2=82.54; p<0.010.001 
No  17  0.51 (-0.05 to 1.07)  0.066 
Configuration         
AET first  13  0.45 (-0.05 to 0.96)  Q = 147.56; I2=80.45; p<0.010.08 
SRT first  10  0.33 (0.13 to 0.48)  <0.001 
Circle  0.54 (0.17 to 0.91)  0.005 
Separation  0.23 (-0.06 to 0.51  0.119 
Unclear  0.25 (-0.05 to 0.55)  0.106 
Duration, min         
Short (30–45)  12  0.28 (0.04 to 0.53)  Q = 145.63; I2=82.75; p<0.010.024 
Medium (60)  17  0.21 (0.06 to 0.35)  0.006 
Long (90)  1.03 (-0.16 to 2.22)  0.089 
Frequency, day/week         
Low (1–2)  0.29 (0.13 to 0.48)  Q = 146.40; I2=81.71; p<0.010.002 
Medium (3–4)  20  0.32 (0.02 to 0.63)  0.036 
High (≥5)  0.42 (0.14 to 0.70)  0.004 
Length, week         
Short (4–12)  13  0.28 (0.11 to 0.44)  Q =182.96; I2=82.54 p<0.010.001 
Medium (13–26)  16  0.21 (0.05 to 0.37)  0.011 
Long (>26)  0.75 (-0.13 to 1.63)  0.093 
Cognitive moderators
Cognitive tasks         
MMSE  15  0.33 (0.19 to 0.47)  Q =182.96; I2=82.54 p<0.01<0.001 
3MSE  0.53 (-0.93 to 1.99)  0.479 
ADAS-Cog  0.37 (0.12 to 0.61)  0.004 
GCS  0.05 (-0.05 to 0.15)  0.316 
MoCA  0.53 (0.23 to 0.84)  0.001 
Other cognitive tests  0.22 (0.01 to 0.43)  0.043 
Comparison moderators
Comparison type         
Health education  0.68 (-0.39 to 1.75)  Q = 143.08; I2=85.10 p<0.010.215 
No exercise  0.44 (0.19 to 0.69)  0.001 
Other exercises  0.07 (-0.23 to 0.38)  0.643 
Routine activity  0.30 (0.02 to 0.57)  0.033 
Routine treatment / care  0.45 (0.05 to 0.85)  0.027 
Social activity  0.18 (-0.20 to 0.55)  0.358 

k: number of effect sizes, CI: confidence intervals, min: minutes, AET: aerobic / endurance / cardiovascular / cardiorespiratory exercise training, SRT: strength / resistance exercise training.

***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05

Participants moderators

Participants aged over 65 exhibited significant effects (g = 0.33, 95% CI: 0.14 to 0.51; p < 0.05), whereas those aged 18 to 65 did not (p = 0.099), indicating greater cognitive benefits from exercise in older adults. Clinical populations demonstrated significant effects (g = 0.28, 95% CI: 0.11 to 0.46; p < 0.001), while healthy populations exhibited a non-significant trend (p = 0.063), and mixed-status populations did not show significant effects (p = 0.33).

Exercise moderators

Both exercises performed within the same session (g = 0.34, 95% CI: 0.10 to 0.66; p = 0.007) and CT supplemented with additional interventions (g = 0.26, 95% CI: 0.10 to 0.42; p = 0.001) exhibited significant effects. SRT performed first (g = 0.33, 95% CI: 0.13 to 0.48; p < 0.001) and circuit training (g = 0.54, 95% CI: 0.17 to 0.91; p = 0.005) exhibited significant effects, whereas AET performed first (p = 0.08) and training separation (p = 0.119) were non-significant. Both short-duration (g = 0.28, 95% CI: 0.04 to 0.53; p = 0.024) and medium-duration (g = 0.21, 95% CI: 0.06 to 0.35; p = 0.006) interventions demonstrated significant improvements. Exercise frequency yielded significant estimates across all three categories: low (g = 0.29, 95% CI: 0.13 to 0.48; p = 0.002), medium (g = 0.32, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.63; p = 0.036), and high (g = 0.42, 95% CI: 0.14 to 0.70; p = 0.004). Medium-length interventions (13–26 weeks) demonstrated a significant ES (g = 0.21, 95% CI: 0.05 to 0.37; p = 0.011). Short interventions (4–12 weeks) exhibited a highly significant effect (g = 0.28, 95% CI: 0.11 to 0.44; p = 0.001). In contrast, long interventions (>26 weeks) exhibited a large effect size (g = 0.75, 95% CI: -0.13 to 1.63) but did not achieve statistical significance (p = 0.093), suggesting variability in their efficacy or the necessity for larger sample sizes to attain significance.

Cognitive moderators

Cognitive tasks varied in effectiveness, with the MMSE, ADAS-Cog, and MoCA demonstrating significant effects (p < 0.001, p = 0.004, and p = 0.001, respectively), whereas the 3MSE and GCS did not achieve significance (p = 0.479 and p = 0.316, respectively). These findings suggest that certain assessments are more sensitive to exercise-related cognitive changes.

Comparison moderators

Comparison types significantly influenced intervention outcomes: Health education control exhibited a large ES (g = 0.68), although it was non-significant (95% CI: -0.39 to 1.75; p = 0.215); no exercise control demonstrated a significant moderate ES (g = 0.44, 95% CI: 0.19 to 0.69; p = 0.001); other exercise controls yielded a small and non-significant ES (g = 0.07, 95% CI: -0.23 to 0.38; p = 0.643); routine activity control indicated a significant ES (g = 0.30, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.57; p = 0.033); routine treatment/care control exhibited a significant moderate ES (g = 0.45, 95% CI: 0.05 to 0.85; p = 0.027); social activity control was non-significant (g = 0.18, 95% CI: -0.20 to 0.55; p = 0.358). These results underscore the significance of designing appropriate comparison conditions in intervention studies.

Meta‑regression analysis and subgroup analyses

The results of the univariate moderator analyses are presented in Table 4. The meta-regression analysis indicated that single-group sample size (R2 = 0.51, p = 0.031) and health status (R2 = 0.495, p = 0.045) significantly contributed to the heterogeneity of intervention effects. Consequently, subgroup analyses were conducted on these variables. The results of the predefined subgroup analyses are illustrated in Fig. 5 and Fig. 6. The subgroup analyses indicated that a sample size of n ≥ 50 and healthy populations were potential sources of heterogeneity (Q = 117.89 - 179.79, p < 0.01; I² = 94.27 - 98%), suggesting that the magnitude of treatment effects or overall health of participants may be influenced by the size of the study population.

Table 4.

Meta-regression analysis for potential heterogeneity.

Covariates  SE (95% CI)  R2  p value 
Age (18–65 years, >65 years)  36  0.25 (-0.47 to 0.54)  0.03  0.69 
Gender (Female>50%)  33  0.24 (-0.34 to 0.63)  0.144  0.550 
Health status (Healthy vs. Clinical)  42  0.24 (0.01 to 0.98)  0.495*  0.045 
Single-group sample size (n>50 or n≤50)  39  0.20 (0.05 to 0.86)  0.51*  0.031 
Comparison type  41  0.08 (-0.15 to 0.19)  -0.017  0.864 
Additional exercise (Yes or No)  47  0.38 (-0.09 to 1.55)  0.961  0.079 
Length (<16 wks, 16–26 wks, ≥26 wks)  47  0.25 (-0.35 to 0.74)  0.637  0.16 
Frequency (1–2, 3–4, ≥5)  37  0.36 (-1.14 to 0.44)  -0.045  0.893 
Same session (Yes or No)  36  0.46 (-0.89 to 1.10)  0.076  0.93 
Configuration (SRT first or AET first)  27  0.24 (-0.44 to 0.61)  -0.149  0.623 
Cognitive tasks  48  0.11 (-0.21 to 0.26)  -0.161  0.292 

k: number of effect sizes, SE: standard error; CI: confidence intervals; R2: regression coefficient; wks: weeks; AET: aerobic / endurance / cardiovascular / cardiorespiratory exercise training, SRT: strength / resistance exercise training.

***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01,

p < 0.05

Fig. 5.

Effects of concurrent training on cognitive outcomes: subgroup analysis by single-group sample sizes. CI, confidence interval; IV, inverse variance; SD, standard deviation.

(1.02MB).
Fig. 6.

Effects of concurrent training on cognitive outcomes: subgroup analysis by participants’ health status. CI, confidence interval; IV, inverse variance; SD, standard deviation.

(0.91MB).
Discussion

This meta-analysis represents the first comprehensive evaluation of the effects of CT on cognitive health, incorporating data from 35 RCTs involving 5734 participants. The main findings indicate that CT effectively enhances overall cognitive function, though its effects are not superior to those of single-modality training. Specifically, older adults (>65 years) and clinical populations demonstrated the greatest cognitive benefits from CT interventions compared to younger adults (18–65 years) and healthy populations. In terms of exercise configurations, the most pronounced cognitive effects were observed with supplemental interventions, sessions lasting 30 to 60 minutes, intervention durations of 4 to 26 weeks, and the prioritization of SRT over AET (intra-session SRT prior to AET) within a single session. Furthermore, meta-regression analysis suggested that heterogeneity could stem from sample size and participants’ health status, with healthy populations and sample sizes of ≥50 partially accounting for the observed variability. Subgroup analyses did not reveal statistically significant positive effects of CT in healthy populations.

The superior cognitive benefits of CT in older adults (g = 0.33) align with the neuroprotective properties of myokine signaling (Coelho-Junior et al., 2022), which may compensate for age-related declines in cardiovascular-driven BDNF production (Erickson et al., 2011). In contrast, younger populations showed non-significant responses (p = 0.099), potentially due to their higher baseline cognitive resilience (Opdebeeck et al., 2016; Stern, 2002). The critical role of exercise order (SRT preceding AET) supports the "anabolic priming" hypothesis(Timmons et al., 2018): SRT primes the body's protein synthesis pathways, while subsequent AET takes advantage of remaining metabolic flexibility to sustain the release of neurotrophic factors. Conversely, reversed order may induce premature glycogen depletion, limiting AET's neuroplasticity effects (Fyfe et al., 2014).

Main effects

The overall findings indicate moderate effects of CT on global cognition. This finding is consistent with previous research (Bae et al., 2019; Huang et al., 2022) which suggests that combined exercise modalities improve cognitive health, particularly in older adults (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003). The observed positive effects may be attributed to the synergistic benefits of aerobic and resistance training, which enhance neuroplasticity, increase cerebral blood flow, and promote the release of neurotrophic factors [i.e., BDNF, Insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1)] and myokines (i.e., Irisin and Cathepsin B) (Cassilhas et al., 2007; Cotman & Berchtold, 2002; Kim et al., 2019; Pereira et al., 2007; Tsai et al., 2019). These cognitive improvements may be mediated by enhanced synaptic plasticity and reduced neuroinflammation, as suggested by previous studies (Voss et al., 2013). Additionally, the combination of aerobic and resistance training may offer unique benefits by targeting multiple aspects of health, such as cardiovascular fitness and muscular strength, both of which are critical for maintaining physical and cognitive function in older populations (Iso-Markku et al., 2024). In summary, the cognitive benefits of CT are driven by a complex interplay of physiological and neurological mechanisms. Future research should further investigate these mechanisms in multimodal versus single-modality exercise, with the goal of informing targeted exercise prescriptions to optimize cognitive function across the lifespan.

Moderator analyses

Firstly, the age of participants significantly influences cognitive improvement. Adults aged 65 years and older demonstrated substantial cognitive benefits from exercise, while those aged 18 to 65 did not show significant effects (p = 0.099). This conclusion is corroborated by previous studies (Hertzog et al., 2008; Kramer & Erickson, 2007b; Paterson & Warburton, 2010), which suggest that cognitive decline associated with aging may heighten sensitivity to exercise-induced adaptations. Another possible explanation for this is the enhanced potentially for neuroplasticity and cognitive reserve in aging population (Stern, 2002). Secondly, prioritizing SRT within the exercise programs markedly promotes cognitive function, whereas prioritizing AET fail to achieve statistically significant improvements. This may be elucidated by the theory of ‘cognitive resources,’ which posits that CT modalities compete for limited neural resources, potentially resulting in cognitive fatigue (McMorris, 2020; Tomporowski & Pesce, 2019). Specifically, intra-session SRT prior to AET may be associated with stronger neuromuscular activation (Carroll et al., 2001) and a shorter recovery window (Hillman et al., 2003), while prioritizing AET, characterized by prolonged endurance exercise, may induce cognitive fatigue (Y.-K. Chang et al., 2012), weaken interaction effects (McMorris et al., 2011), and create differences in recovery periods, ultimately diminish cognitive benefits (McNerney & Radvansky, 2015). Another source of uncertainty highlighted by both our results and previous research, moreover, may interfere with each other due to neural competition between AET and SRT (Kramer & Erickson, 2007a; Lan et al., 2018). Animal studies suggest that SRT may produce negative effect on cognitive improvement induced by AET (Lan et al., 2018), which could explain the inconsistent improvements observed when AET precedes SRT (p = 0.08). Consequently, future research should investigate the optimal sequence of exercise to maximise cognitive adaptations.

Our findings indicate that short- to medium-term interventions (4–26 weeks) demonstrate positive effects on cognitive improvement. These outcomes may reflect the adaptive responses to prolonged exercise, where participants acclimate to repeated stimuli, resulting in diminished impact over time (Kraemer & Ratamess, 2005). The impacts of short- and medium-term interventions is closely related to participants’ motivation and engagement (McAuley & Rudolph, 1995), as these interventions can sustain enthusiasm, provide cognitive feedback, and enhance adherence due to appropriate length (Teixeira et al., 2012). In contrast, longer interventions may increase feelings of fatigue or reduce willingness to participate due to a lack of novelty, thereby affecting cognitive improvement outcomes (Kahn et al., 2002). These results highlight the necessity of larger sample sizes and carefully tailored interventions to capture meaningful changes in cognition. Short- to medium-term programs should therefore be prioritised to optimize cognitive benefits. Session duration results suggest that 30 to 60 minutes is optimal for enhancing cognitive function. Current research suggests that moderate-duration exercise improves neural plasticity by increasing cerebral blood flow, promoting the release of neurotrophic factors, and enhancing metabolic health (Erickson et al., 2011; Kramer & Erickson, 2007b; Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010). In contrast, 90-minute exercise sessions may lead to fatigue, reduced engagement, and diminished cognitive performance (Boksem & Tops, 2008; Ehlers et al., 2017). Longer durations do not necessarily yield additional cognitive benefits than shorter and more efficient workouts, consistent with the principle of diminishing returns (Kramer & Erickson, 2007b). Thus, interventions targeting cognitive health should prioritise exercise durations of 30 to 60 minutes for optimal benefits.

Assessment tools, including the MMSE, ADAS-Cog, and MoCA, exhibit high sensitivity to cognitive changes induced by exercise (p < 0.001). These tools are widely utilized among older adults because they assess global cognitive function across multiple domains, capturing subtle yet meaningful improvements (Folstein et al., 1975; Nasreddine et al., 2005). The MMSE and MoCA also evaluate language, visuospatial abilities, and attention, rendering them suitable for assessing the comprehensive effects of exercise on brain health. Utilizing sensitive assessment tools is crucial for detecting significant cognitive changes, particularly in complex domains such as executive function and memory (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003). Therefore, practitioners must exercise caution when selecting assessment tools for intervention studies related to cognitive health to ensure that genuine cognitive changes are accurately captured.

Finally, distinguishing between active and passive control conditions is vital for interpreting the effects of CT on cognitive health. Active controls, including health education, exercise interventions, and social activities, inherently involve cognitive engagement or physical activity, thereby influencing outcomes differently from passive controls. Structured exercise provides superior cognitive benefits compared to passive controls, indicating that it offers cognitive advantages beyond routine activities. These findings partially align with existing research (Northey et al., 2018), which indicates that exercise interventions are notably effective compared to passive control groups (e.g., wait-list), while the effects are not significant when compared to active control groups (e.g., stretching training). Thus, understanding the relationship between active and passive control conditions is crucial for optimizing the cognitive benefits of exercise-induced effects.

Subgroup analysis and meta-regression

The substantial heterogeneity (I² = 86%) identified in our meta-analysis is noteworthy. Meta-regression and subgroup analyses revealed that variability was driven by factors such as participant age, clinical status, and exercise characteristics. Older adults and clinical populations, who are at greater risk for cognitive decline, showed the most pronounced cognitive improvements. In contrast, healthy populations exhibited non-significant trends, indicating that while exercise may have a protective effect, it does not necessarily yield significant cognitive gains in individuals without pre-existing cognitive impairments.

Psychosocial moderators: Mental health, socioeconomics, and personality

Beyond physiological mechanisms, psychosocial factors may critically shape the cognitive outcomes of CT. First, emerging evidence suggests that CT may uniquely benefit individuals with depression or schizophrenia by addressing both cognitive deficits and psychomotor retardation. For example, SRT enhances dopaminergic signaling, which is often dysregulated in schizophrenia (Firth et al., 2017), whereas AET ameliorates hippocampal atrophy in depression (Pajonk et al., 2010). Future trials should prioritise standardised assessments of psychiatric symptoms (e.g., PHQ-9 for depression) to disentangle the bidirectional effects of exercise on mental and cognitive health (Kandola et al., 2019; Kroenke et al., 2001). While our analysis did not directly assess mental health outcomes, prior trials suggest that CT may synergistically improve both cognition and emotional well-being (Schuch, Vancampfort, Richards, et al., 2016). Second, socioeconomic determinants such as education and income likely influence intervention efficacy (Tomporowski & Pesce, 2019). Higher socioeconomic status correlates with greater access to supervised training facilities and health literacy, which may explain variability in adherence rates across studies (Stringhini et al., 2017). Third, personality traits—particularly conscientiousness and openness—may predict long-term exercise adherence, thereby indirectly moderating cognitive gains (McCrae & Sutin, 2018). Future research should explicitly measure these variables to disentangle their roles.

Limitations and future research perspectives

The generalisability of these findings is constrained by several limitations. Firstly, significant heterogeneity exists within the sample. The 35 included RCTs featured participants spanning a broad age range (22.4 - 92.1 years), with varying health statuses and sample sizes. This population diversity may contribute to the observed heterogeneity in the results. Secondly, differences in the design and methodology—such as training configuration, intervention duration, and types of exercise—may affect the comparability and generalizability of the results. Additionally, some studies lacked detailed reporting on exercise intensity and load, limiting deeper insights into the intervention effects. Fourthly, despite subgroup analyses, the substantial heterogeneity in exercise protocols (e.g., intensity, session order, and progression) across studies may confound the interpretation of optimal CT configurations. Fifthly, our study did not systematically evaluate the role of psychosocial factors (e.g., baseline mental health, socioeconomic status, or personality traits) and psychiatric comorbidities (e.g., depression, anxiety) due to inconsistent reporting in the included trials. Finally, the absence of mechanistic evidence (e.g., neuroimaging, biomarkers) restricts our understanding of sustained cognitive benefits and underlying pathways. Notably, current research predominantly focuses on older adults, while studies investigating the cognitive effects of comprehensive exercise interventions in children and adolescents remain scarce. Thus, these results should be interpreted with caution. Future high-quality RCTs are necessary to investigate the effects of exercise interventions across age groups and to validate the long-term impacts of various exercise combinations, thereby enabling the development of more targeted exercise prescriptions. The integration of neuroimaging and biomarker analyses will enhance our understanding of how exercise influences brain structure and function, elucidating the physiological and psychological mechanisms through which exercise promotes cognitive health and advancing the development of sports medicine strategies.

Practical application

Our findings offer actionable insights for designing cognitive health prevention programs. First, prioritizing CT with a focus on SRT before AET within a single session may maximise cognitive benefits, particularly for older adults and clinical populations at risk of neurodegeneration. Public health initiatives could integrate CT into community-based exercise programs, emphasizing supervised sessions of 30–60 minutes, 3–4 times per week, over 13–26 weeks. Second, healthcare providers may adopt CT as a non-pharmacological intervention to delay cognitive decline in individuals with MCI or early-stage dementia, given its low cost and minimal side effects. Finally, policymakers should consider promoting combined exercise regimens (e.g., combining strength and balance training with cognitive challenges) in national guidelines to enhance both physical and cognitive resilience across the lifespan.

Conclusions

Concurrent training yield moderate improvements in cognitive health, particularly among older adults aged over 65 and clinical populations. Prioritizing resistance training, implementing short-to-medium term (4–26 weeks), and maintaining session durations of 30–60 minutes are crucial for optimizing cognitive benefits. Employing sensitive assessment tools is vital for accurately assessing cognitive changes. Further studies into optimal training protocols and multiple domains, will deepen our understanding of exercise as a form of preventive medicine for cognition. Future prevention programs should prioritise the integration of concurrent training into routine healthcare for aging populations, particularly through community centres and digital platforms that provide accessible, structured exercise protocols. By aligning exercise prescriptions with the parameters identified in this study (e.g., session duration, frequency, and exercise sequence), stakeholders can optimize cognitive health outcomes while addressing the growing burden of neurodegenerative diseases.

Availability of data and material

All data generated or analyzed throughout this systematic review with meta-analysis were included in the main article and its supplementary materials.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the researchers contacted who provided data and information about their work.

References
[A'Naja et al., 2024]
M.N. A'Naja, A. Batrakoulis, S.M. Camhi, C. McAvoy, J.S. Sansone, R. Reed.
2025 ACSM worldwide fitness trends: future directions of the health and fitness industry.
ACSM's Health & Fitness Journal, 28 (2024), pp. 11-25
[Al-Mhanna et al., 2024]
S.B. Al-Mhanna, A. Batrakoulis, W.S.W. Ghazali, M. Mohamed, A. Aldayel, M.H. Alhussain, S. …Elkholi.
Effects of combined aerobic and resistance training on glycemic control, blood pressure, inflammation, cardiorespiratory fitness and quality of life in patients with type 2 diabetes and overweight/obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Peer Journal, 12 (2024), pp. e17525
[Al-Mhanna et al., 2023]
S.B. Al-Mhanna, S. Rocha-Rodriguesc, M. Mohamed, A. Batrakoulis, M.I. Aldhahi, H.A. Afolabi, B.D. Abubakar.
Effects of combined aerobic exercise and diet on cardiometabolic health in patients with obesity and type 2 diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
BMC Sports Science, Medicine and Rehabilitation, 15 (2023), pp. 165
[Amatriain-Fernández et al., 2021]
S. Amatriain-Fernández, M. Ezquerro García-Noblejas, H. Budde.
Effects of chronic exercise on the inhibitory control of children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 31 (2021), pp. 1196-1208
[Ansai and Rebelatto, 2015]
J.H. Ansai, J.R. Rebelatto.
Effect of two physical exercise protocols on cognition and depressive symptoms in oldest-old people: A randomised controlled trial.
Geriatrics & gerontology international, 15 (2015), pp. 1127-1134
[Arrieta et al., 2020]
H. Arrieta, C. Rezola-Pardo, M. Kortajarena, G. Hervás, J. Gil, J.J. Yanguas, A. …Rodriguez-Larrad.
The impact of physical exercise on cognitive and affective functions and serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in nursing home residents: a randomized controlled trial.
[Babaei et al., 2013]
P. Babaei, K. Azali Alamdari, B. Soltani Tehrani, A. Damirchi.
Effect of six weeks of endurance exercise and following detraining on serum brain derived neurotrophic factor and memory performance in middle aged males with metabolic syndrome.
The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 53 (2013), pp. 437-443
[Bae et al., 2019]
S. Bae, S. Lee, S. Lee, S. Jung, K. Makino, K. Harada, H. …Shimada.
The effect of a multicomponent intervention to promote community activity on cognitive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial.
Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 42 (2019), pp. 164-169
[Baechle and Earle, 2008]
T.R. Baechle, R.W. Earle.
Essentials of strength training and conditioning.
Human kinetics, (2008),
[Ballesteros et al., 2024]
S. Ballesteros, M. Audifren, A. Badache, V. Belkin, C.D. Giannaki, A. Kaltsatou, Y. …Netz.
Effects of chronic physical exercise on executive functions and episodic memory in clinical and healthy older adult populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol.
Systematic reviews, 13 (2024), pp. 98
[Barnes et al., 2013]
D.E. Barnes, W. Santos-Modesitt, G. Poelke, A.F. Kramer, C. Castro, L.E. Middleton, K. Yaffe.
The Mental Activity and eXercise (MAX) trial: a randomized controlled trial to enhance cognitive function in older adults.
JAMA internal medicine, 173 (2013), pp. 797-804
[Batrakoulis and Fatouros, 2022]
A. Batrakoulis, I.G. Fatouros.
Psychological adaptations to high-intensity interval training in overweight and obese adults: A topical review.
[Batrakoulis et al., 2022]
A. Batrakoulis, A.Z. Jamurtas, G.S. Metsios, K. Perivoliotis, G. Liguori, Y. Feito, P. …Krustrup.
Comparative efficacy of 5 exercise types on cardiometabolic health in overweight and obese adults: a systematic review and network meta-analysis of 81 randomized controlled trials.
Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes, 15 (2022),
[Beard et al., 2016]
J.R. Beard, A. Officer, I.A. De Carvalho, R. Sadana, A.M. Pot, J.-P. Michel, W.R. Mahanani.
The World report on ageing and health: a policy framework for healthy ageing.
The Lancet, 387 (2016), pp. 2145-2154
[Begg and Mazumdar, 1994]
C.B. Begg, M. Mazumdar.
Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias.
Biometrics, (1994), pp. 1088-1101
[Bell et al., 2019]
K.E. Bell, H. Fang, T. Snijders, D.J. Allison, M.A. Zulyniak, A. Chabowski, J.J. …Heisz.
A multi-ingredient nutritional supplement in combination with resistance exercise and high-intensity interval training improves cognitive function and increases N-3 index in healthy older men: a randomized controlled trial.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 11 (2019), pp. 107
[Best et al., 2015]
J.R. Best, B.K. Chiu, C.L. Hsu, L.S. Nagamatsu, T. Liu-Ambrose.
Long-term effects of resistance exercise training on cognition and brain volume in older women: results from a randomized controlled trial.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21 (2015), pp. 745-756
[Biazus-Sehn, Schuch, Firth and de Souza Stigger, 2020]
L.F. Biazus-Sehn, F.B. Schuch, J. Firth, F. de Souza Stigger.
Effects of physical exercise on cognitive function of older adults with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 89 (2020),
[Blair, 2002]
C. Blair.
School readiness: integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry.
American Psychologist, 57 (2002), pp. 111
[Boksem and Tops, 2008]
M.A. Boksem, M. Tops.
Mental fatigue: costs and benefits.
Brain Research Reviews, 59 (2008), pp. 125-139
[Borg, 1998]
G. Borg.
Borg's perceived exertion and pain scales.
Human kinetics, (1998),
[Bossers et al., 2014]
W.J. Bossers, E.J. Scherder, F. Boersma, T. Hortobagyi, L.H. van der Woude, M.J. van Heuvelen.
Feasibility of a combined aerobic and strength training program and its effects on cognitive and physical function in institutionalized dementia patients. A pilot study.
[Bossers et al., 2015]
W.J. Bossers, L.H. van der Woude, F. Boersma, T. Hortobagyi, E.J. Scherder, M.J. van Heuvelen.
A 9-week aerobic and strength training program improves cognitive and motor function in patients with dementia: A randomized, controlled trial.
The American journal of Geriatric Psychiatry : Official Journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, 23 (2015), pp. 1106-1116
[Callisaya et al., 2017]
M. Callisaya, R. Daly, J. Sharman, D. Bruce, T. Davis, T. Greenaway, T. …Phan.
Feasibility of a multi-modal exercise program on cognition in older adults with type 2 diabetes–a pilot randomized controlled trial.
BMC Geriatrics, 17 (2017), pp. 1-10
[Cammisuli, Innocenti, Franzoni and Pruneti, 2017]
D. Cammisuli, A. Innocenti, F. Franzoni, C. Pruneti.
Aerobic exercise effects upon cognition in Mild Cognitive Impairment: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials.
Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 155 (2017), pp. 55-63
[Cancela Carral and Ayán Pérez, 2008]
J. Cancela Carral, C. Ayán Pérez.
Effects of high-intensity combined training on women over 65.
Gerontology, 53 (2008), pp. 340-346
[Carroll, Riek and Carson, 2001]
T.J. Carroll, S. Riek, R.G. Carson.
Neural adaptations to resistance training: implications for movement control.
Sports Medicine, 31 (2001), pp. 829-840
[Carta et al., 2021]
M.G. Carta, G. Cossu, E. Pintus, R. Zaccheddu, O. Callia, G. Conti, G. …Mura.
Moderate exercise improves cognitive function in healthy elderly people: results of a randomized controlled trial.
Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in Mental Health: CP & EMH, 17 (2021), pp. 75
[Cassilhas et al., 2012]
R.C. Cassilhas, K.S. Lee, J. Fernandes, M.G.M.d. Oliveira, S. Tufik, R. Meeusen, M. De Mello.
Spatial memory is improved by aerobic and resistance exercise through divergent molecular mechanisms.
Neuroscience, 202 (2012), pp. 309-317
[Cassilhas et al., 2007]
R.C. Cassilhas, V.A. Viana, V. Grassmann, R.T. Santos, R.F. Santos, S. Tufik, M.T. Mello.
The impact of resistance exercise on the cognitive function of the elderly.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 39 (2007), pp. 1401-1407
[Cavalcante et al., 2020]
B.R. Cavalcante, M.F. de Souza, R.S. Falck, T. Liu-Ambrose, D.G. Behm, A.C.R. Pitangui, R.C. de Araujo.
Effects of resistance exercise with instability on cognitive function (REI Study): A proof-of-concept randomized controlled trial in older adults with cognitive complaints.
Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 77 (2020), pp. 227-239
[Cerrillo-Urbina et al., 2015]
A.J. Cerrillo-Urbina, A. García-Hermoso, M. Sánchez-López, M.J. Pardo-Guijarro, J. Santos Gómez, V. Martínez-Vizcaíno.
The effects of physical exercise in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized control trials.
Child: Care, Health and Development, 41 (2015), pp. 779-788
[Chang et al., 2012]
Y.-K. Chang, J.D. Labban, J.I. Gapin, J.L. Etnier.
The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis.
Brain Research, 1453 (2012), pp. 87-101
[Chang and Etnier, 2009]
Y.K. Chang, J.L. Etnier.
Effects of an acute bout of localized resistance exercise on cognitive performance in middle-aged adults: A randomized controlled trial study.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10 (2009), pp. 19-24
[Chang et al., 2012]
Y.K. Chang, P.W. Ku, P.D. Tomporowski, F.T. Chen, C.C. Huang.
Effects of acute resistance exercise on late-middle-age adults' goal planning.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 44 (2012), pp. 1773-1779
[Chang et al., 2012]
Y.K. Chang, C.Y. Pan, F.T. Chen, C.L. Tsai, C.C. Huang.
Effect of resistance-exercise training on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a review.
Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 20 (2012), pp. 497-517
[Chen et al., 2020]
F.-T. Chen, J.L. Etnier, K.-H. Chan, P.-K. Chiu, T.-M. Hung, Y.-K. Chang.
Effects of exercise training interventions on executive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Sports Medicine, 50 (2020), pp. 1451-1467
[Chow et al., 2021]
Z.-S. Chow, A.T. Moreland, H. Macpherson, W.-P. Teo.
The central mechanisms of resistance training and its effects on cognitive function.
Sports Medicine, 51 (2021), pp. 2483-2506
[Ciria et al., 2023]
L.F. Ciria, R. Román-Caballero, M.A. Vadillo, D. Holgado, A. Luque-Casado, P. Perakakis, D. Sanabria.
An umbrella review of randomized control trials on the effects of physical exercise on cognition.
Nature Human Behaviour, 7 (2023), pp. 928-941
[Cochran, 1954]
W.G. Cochran.
The combination of estimates from different experiments.
Biometrics, 10 (1954), pp. 101-129
[Coelho-Junior et al., 2022]
H. Coelho-Junior, E. Marzetti, R. Calvani, A. Picca, H. Arai, M. Uchida.
Resistance training improves cognitive function in older adults with different cognitive status: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Aging & Mental Health, 26 (2022), pp. 213-224
[Coffey and Hawley, 2017]
V.G. Coffey, J.A. Hawley.
Concurrent exercise training: do opposites distract?.
The Journal of Physiology, 595 (2017), pp. 2883-2896
[Colcombe and Kramer, 2003]
S. Colcombe, A.F. Kramer.
Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study.
Psychological Science, 14 (2003), pp. 125-130
[Costigan et al., 2016]
S.A. Costigan, N. Eather, R.C. Plotnikoff, C.H. Hillman, D.R. Lubans.
High-intensity interval training for cognitive and mental health in adolescents.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 48 (2016), pp. 1985-1993
[Cotman and Berchtold, 2002]
C.W. Cotman, N.C. Berchtold.
Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity.
Trends in Neurosciences, 25 (2002), pp. 295-301
[da Silveira Langoni et al., 2019]
C. da Silveira Langoni, T. de Lima Resende, A.B. Barcellos, B. Cecchele, M.S. Knob, T. do Nascimento Silva, C.H.A. Schwanke.
Effect of exercise on cognition, conditioning, muscle endurance, and balance in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial.
Journal of Geriatric Physical Therapy, 42 (2019), pp. E15-E22
[Davis et al., 2007]
C.L. Davis, P.D. Tomporowski, C.A. Boyle, J.L. Waller, P.H. Miller, J.A. Naglieri, M. Gregoski.
Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children's cognitive functioning: a randomized controlled trial.
[de Asteasu et al., 2017]
M.L.S. de Asteasu, N. Martinez-Velilla, F. Zambom-Ferraresi, A. Casas-Herrero, M. Izquierdo.
Role of physical exercise on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a systematic review of randomized clinical trials.
Ageing Research Reviews, 37 (2017), pp. 117-134
[de Oliveira Silva et al., 2019]
F. de Oliveira Silva, J.V. Ferreira, J. Plácido, P. Sant'Anna, J. Araújo, V. Marinho, A.C. …Deslandes.
Three months of multimodal training contributes to mobility and executive function in elderly individuals with mild cognitive impairment, but not in those with Alzheimer's disease: a randomized controlled trial.
[de Souto Barreto et al., 2017]
P. de Souto Barreto, M. Cesari, P. Denormandie, D. Armaingaud, B. Vellas, Y. Rolland.
Exercise or social intervention for Nursing Home residents with dementia: A pilot randomized, controlled trial.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 65 (2017), pp. E123-E129
[Deckers et al., 2024]
K. Deckers, M.D. Zwan, L.M. Soons, L. Waterink, S. Beers, S. van Houdt, F.-N. …consortium.
A multidomain lifestyle intervention to maintain optimal cognitive functioning in Dutch older adults-study design and baseline characteristics of the FINGER-NL randomized controlled trial.
Alzheimers Research & Therapy, 16 (2024), pp. 126
[Diamond and Lee, 2011]
A. Diamond, K. Lee.
Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old.
Science (New York, N.Y.), 333 (2011), pp. 959-964
[Dunsky et al., 2017]
A. Dunsky, M. Abu-Rukun, S. Tsuk, T. Dwolatzky, R. Carasso, Y. Netz.
The effects of a resistance vs. an aerobic single session on attention and executive functioning in adults.
PloS one, 12 (2017),
[Duval and Tweedie, 2000]
S. Duval, R. Tweedie.
Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis.
Biometrics, 56 (2000), pp. 455-463
[Eckardt, Braun and Kibele, 2020]
N. Eckardt, C. Braun, A. Kibele.
Instability resistance training improves working memory, processing speed and response inhibition in healthy older adults: A double-blinded randomized controlled trial.
Scientific Reports, 10 (2020), pp. 2506
[Egger, Smith, Schneider and Minder, 1997]
M. Egger, G.D. Smith, M. Schneider, C. Minder.
Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test.
BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 315 (1997), pp. 629-634
[Ehlers et al., 2017]
D.K. Ehlers, S. Aguiñaga, J. Cosman, J. Severson, A.F. Kramer, E. McAuley.
The effects of physical activity and fatigue on cognitive performance in breast cancer survivors.
Breast cancer Research and Treatment, 165 (2017), pp. 699-707
[Engeroff et al., 2019]
T. Engeroff, D. Niederer, L. Vogt, W. Banzer.
Intensity and workload related dose-response effects of acute resistance exercise on domain-specific cognitive function and affective response–a four-armed randomized controlled crossover trial.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 43 (2019), pp. 55-63
[Erickson et al., 2019]
K.I. Erickson, C. Hillman, C.M. Stillman, R.M. Ballard, B. Bloodgood, D.E. Conroy, K.E. Powell.
Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: a review of the 2018 physical activity guidelines.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 51 (2019), pp. 1242
[Erickson et al., 2011]
K.I. Erickson, M.W. Voss, R.S. Prakash, C. Basak, A. Szabo, L. Chaddock, S.M. White.
Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108 (2011), pp. 3017-3022
[Espeland et al., 2017]
M.A. Espeland, K. Lipska, M.E. Miller, J. Rushing, R.A. Cohen, J. Verghese, S.N. …Blair.
Effects of physical activity intervention on physical and cognitive function in sedentary adults with and without diabetes.
Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences, 72 (2017), pp. 861-866
[Firth et al., 2017]
J. Firth, B. Stubbs, S. Rosenbaum, D. Vancampfort, B. Malchow, F. Schuch, A.R. Yung.
Aerobic exercise improves cognitive functioning in people with schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43 (2017), pp. 546-556
[Folstein et al., 1975]
M.F. Folstein, S.E. Folstein, P.R. McHugh.
Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
Journal of psychiatric research, 12 (1975), pp. 189-198
[Fonte et al., 2019]
C. Fonte, N. Smania, A. Pedrinolla, D. Munari, M. Gandolfi, A. Picelli, M. …Venturelli.
Comparison between physical and cognitive treatment in patients with MCI and Alzheimer's disease.
Aging, 11 (2019), pp. 3138-3155
[Fyfe et al., 2014]
J.J. Fyfe, D.J. Bishop, N.K. Stepto.
Interference between concurrent resistance and endurance exercise: molecular bases and the role of individual training variables.
Sports Medicine, 44 (2014), pp. 743-762
[Gallardo-Gómez et al., 2022]
D. Gallardo-Gómez, J. Del Pozo-Cruz, M. Noetel, F. Álvarez-Barbosa, R.M. Alfonso-Rosa, B. Del Pozo Cruz.
Optimal dose and type of exercise to improve cognitive function in older adults: A systematic review and bayesian model-based network meta-analysis of RCTs.
Ageing Research Eeviews, 76 (2022),
[García-Hermoso et al., 2018]
A. García-Hermoso, R. Ramírez-Vélez, R. Ramírez-Campillo, M.D. Peterson, V. Martínez-Vizcaíno.
Concurrent aerobic plus resistance exercise versus aerobic exercise alone to improve health outcomes in paediatric obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
British Journal of Sports Medicine, 52 (2018), pp. 161-166
[Garcia-Soto et al., 2013]
E. Garcia-Soto, M. Lopez de Munain, M. Santibáñez.
Effects of combined aerobic and resistance training on cognition following stroke: a systematic review.
Revista de neurologia, 57 (2013), pp. 535-541
[Ghodrati et al., 2023]
N. Ghodrati, A.H. Haghighi, S.A.H. Kakhak, S. Abbasian, G.S. Goldfield.
Effect of combined exercise training on physical and cognitive function in women with type 2 diabetes.
Canadian journal of diabetes, 47 (2023), pp. 162-170
[Gordon et al., 2018]
B.R. Gordon, C.P. McDowell, M. Hallgren, J.D. Meyer, M. Lyons, M.P. Herring.
Association of efficacy of resistance exercise training with depressive symptoms: meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis of randomized clinical trials.
JAMA psychiatry, 75 (2018), pp. 566-576
[Group, 2004]
G.W. Group.
Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations.
BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 328 (2004), pp. 1490
[Guadagni et al., 2020]
V. Guadagni, L.L. Drogos, A.V. Tyndall, M.H. Davenport, T.J. Anderson, G.A. Eskes, M.J. Poulin.
Aerobic exercise improves cognition and cerebrovascular regulation in older adults.
Neurology, 94 (2020), pp. e2245-e2257
[Hacker et al., 2020]
S. Hacker, W. Banzer, L. Vogt, T. Engeroff.
Acute effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive attention and memory performance: an investigation on duration-based dose-response relations and the impact of increased arousal levels.
Journal of clinical medicine, 9 (2020), pp. 1380
[Han et al., 2023]
C.L. Han, W.S. Sun, D. Zhang, X.S. Xi, R. Zhang, W.J. Gong.
Effects of different aerobic exercises on the global cognitive function of the elderly with mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis.
[Harveson et al., 2016]
A.T. Harveson, J.C. Hannon, T.A. Brusseau, L. Podlog, C. Papadopoulos, L.H. Durrant, K.D. Kang.
Acute effects of 30 minutes resistance and aerobic exercise on cognition in a high school sample.
Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 87 (2016), pp. 214-220
[Haverkamp et al., 2020]
B.F. Haverkamp, R. Wiersma, K. Vertessen, H. van Ewijk, J. Oosterlaan, E. Hartman.
Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive outcomes and academic performance in adolescents and young adults: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Sports Sciences, 38 (2020), pp. 2637-2660
[Hedges and Olkin, 2014]
L.V. Hedges, I. Olkin.
Statistical methods for meta-analysis.
Academic press, (2014),
[Henskens, Nauta, Van Eekeren and Scherder, 2018]
M. Henskens, I.M. Nauta, M.C. Van Eekeren, E.J. Scherder.
Effects of physical activity in nursing home residents with dementia: a randomized controlled trial.
Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders, 46 (2018), pp. 60-80
[Hertzog et al., 2008]
C. Hertzog, A.F. Kramer, R.S. Wilson, U. Lindenberger.
Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced?.
Psychological science in the public interest, 9 (2008), pp. 1-65
[Hickson, 1980]
R.C. Hickson.
Interference of strength development by simultaneously training for strength and endurance.
European journal of applied physiology and occupational physiology, 45 (1980), pp. 255-263
[Higgins and Green, 2008]
Higgins, J.P., & Green, S. (2008). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions.
[Higgins and Thompson, 2002]
J.P. Higgins, S.G. Thompson.
Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis.
Statistics in medicine, 21 (2002), pp. 1539-1558
[Higgins, Thompson, Deeks and Altman, 2003]
J.P. Higgins, S.G. Thompson, J.J. Deeks, D.G. Altman.
Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses.
BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 327 (2003), pp. 557-560
[Hillman et al., 2003]
C.H. Hillman, E.M. Snook, G.J. Jerome.
Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function.
International Journal of psychophysiology, 48 (2003), pp. 307-314
[Huang et al., 2022]
X. Huang, X. Zhao, B. Li, Y. Cai, S. Zhang, Q. Wan, F. Yu.
Comparative efficacy of various exercise interventions on cognitive function in patients with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: A systematic review and network meta-analysis.
Journal of sport and health science, 11 (2022), pp. 212-223
[Huiberts et al., 2024]
R.O. Huiberts, R.C. Wüst, S. van der Zwaard.
Concurrent strength and endurance training: A systematic review and meta-analysis on the impact of sex and training status.
Sports Medicine, 54 (2024), pp. 485-503
[Hung et al., 2013]
T.-M. Hung, C.-L. Tsai, F.-T. Chen, C.-C. Wang, Y.-K. Chang.
The immediate and sustained effects of acute exercise on planning aspect of executive function.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14 (2013), pp. 728-736
[Iso-Markku et al., 2024]
P. Iso-Markku, S. Aaltonen, U.M. Kujala, H.-L. Halme, D. Phipps, K. Knittle, K. …Waller.
Physical activity and cognitive decline among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
JAMA network open, 7 (2024),
[Jia et al., 2020]
L. Jia, Y. Du, L. Chu, Z. Zhang, F. Li, D. Lyu, Y. …Song.
Prevalence, risk factors, and management of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in adults aged 60 years or older in China: a cross-sectional study.
The Lancet public health, 5 (2020), pp. e661-e671
[Kahn et al., 2002]
E.B. Kahn, L.T. Ramsey, R.C. Brownson, G.W. Heath, E.H. Howze, K.E. Powell, P. …Corso.
The effectiveness of interventions to increase physical activity: a systematic review.
American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 22 (2002), pp. 73-107
[Kaliman et al., 2011]
P. Kaliman, M. Párrizas, J.F. Lalanza, A. Camins, R.M. Escorihuela, M. Pallas.
Neurophysiological and epigenetic effects of physical exercise on the aging process.
Ageing Research Reviews, 10 (2011), pp. 475-486
[Kandola et al., 2019]
A. Kandola, G. Ashdown-Franks, J. Hendrikse, C.M. Sabiston, B. Stubbs.
Physical activity and depression: towards understanding the antidepressant mechanisms of physical activity.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 107 (2019), pp. 525-539
[Kim et al., 2019]
S. Kim, J.-Y. Choi, S. Moon, D.-H. Park, H.-B. Kwak, J.-H. Kang.
Roles of myokines in exercise-induced improvement of neuropsychiatric function.
Pflügers Archiv-European Journal of Physiology, 471 (2019), pp. 491-505
[Kraemer and Ratamess, 2005]
W.J. Kraemer, N.A. Ratamess.
Hormonal responses and adaptations to resistance exercise and training.
Sports Medicine, 35 (2005), pp. 339-361
[Kramer and Erickson, 2007a]
A.F. Kramer, K.I. Erickson.
Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function.
Trends in cognitive Sciences, 11 (2007), pp. 342-348
[Kramer and Erickson, 2007b]
A.F. Kramer, K.I. Erickson.
Effects of physical activity on cognition, well-being, and brain: Human interventions.
Alzheimer's & Dementia, 3 (2007), pp. S45-S51
[Kroenke et al., 2001]
K. Kroenke, R.L. Spitzer, J.B. Williams.
The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure.
Journal of general internal medicine, 16 (2001), pp. 606-613
[Lan et al., 2018]
Y. Lan, Z. Huang, Y. Jiang, X. Zhou, J. Zhang, D. Zhang, G. …Hou.
Strength exercise weakens aerobic exercise-induced cognitive improvements in rats.
PloS one, 13 (2018),
[Landrigan et al., 2020]
J.-F. Landrigan, T. Bell, M. Crowe, O.J. Clay, D. Mirman.
Lifting cognition: a meta-analysis of effects of resistance exercise on cognition.
Psychological Research, 84 (2020), pp. 1167-1183
[Lang et al., 2010]
J.W. Lang, M. Kersting, U.R. Hülsheger, J. Lang.
General mental ability, narrower cognitive abilities, and job performance: the perspective of the nested-factors model of cognitive abilities.
Personnel psychology, 63 (2010), pp. 595-640
[Langlois et al., 2013]
F. Langlois, T.T.M. Vu, K. Chassé, G. Dupuis, M.-J. Kergoat, L. Bherer.
Benefits of physical exercise training on cognition and quality of life in frail older adults.
Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68 (2013), pp. 400-404
[Larson and Bruce, 1987]
E.B. Larson, R.A. Bruce.
Health benefits of exercise in an aging society.
Archives of internal medicine, 147 (1987), pp. 353-356
[Lautenschlager et al., 2008]
N.T. Lautenschlager, K.L. Cox, L. Flicker, J.K. Foster, F.M. Van Bockxmeer, J. Xiao, O.P. …Almeida.
Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial.
JAMA, 300 (2008), pp. 1027-1037
[Leach et al., 2016]
H.J. Leach, J.M. Danyluk, K.C. Nishimura, S.N. Culos-Reed.
Benefits of 24 versus 12 weeks of exercise and wellness programming for women undergoing treatment for breast cancer.
Supportive Care in Cancer, 24 (2016), pp. 4597-4606
[Liang et al., 2021]
J. Liang, H. Wang, Y. Zeng, Y. Qu, Q. Liu, F. Zhao, J. …Ying.
Physical exercise promotes brain remodeling by regulating epigenetics, neuroplasticity and neurotrophins.
Reviews in the Neurosciences, 32 (2021), pp. 615-629
[Liberati et al., 2009]
A. Liberati, D.G. Altman, J. Tetzlaff, C. Mulrow, P.C. Gøtzsche, J.P. Ioannidis, D. …Moher.
The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration.
Annals of internal medicine, 151 (2009), pp. W-65
[Liu-Ambrose et al., 2010]
T. Liu-Ambrose, L.S. Nagamatsu, P. Graf, B.L. Beattie, M.C. Ashe, T.C. Handy.
Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial.
Archives of internal medicine, 170 (2010), pp. 170-178
[Lopez-Ortiz et al., 2021]
S. Lopez-Ortiz, P.L. Valenzuela, M.M. Seisdedos, J.S. Morales, T. Vega, A. Castillo-Garcia, A. …Santos-Lozano.
Exercise interventions in Alzheimer's disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials [Review].
Ageing Research Reviews, 72 (2021),
[Lu, 2016]
T. Lu.
Effects of aerobic combined resistance and balance exercise on cognitive function in patients with non-dementia cognitive impairment after stroke.
[Ludyga et al., 2018]
S. Ludyga, M. Gerber, C. Herrmann, S. Brand, U. Pühse.
Chronic effects of exercise implemented during school-break time on neurophysiological indices of inhibitory control in adolescents.
Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 10 (2018), pp. 1-7
[Ludyga et al., 2022]
S. Ludyga, M. Gerber, K. Kamijo.
Exercise types and working memory components during development.
Trends in cognitive Sciences, 26 (2022), pp. 191-203
[Ludyga et al., 2020]
S. Ludyga, M. Gerber, U. Pühse, V.N. Looser, K. Kamijo.
Systematic review and meta-analysis investigating moderators of long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals.
Nature human behaviour, 4 (2020), pp. 603-612
[Lundberg et al., 2022]
T.R. Lundberg, J.F. Feuerbacher, M. Sünkeler, M. Schumann.
The effects of concurrent aerobic and strength training on muscle fiber hypertrophy: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Sports Medicine, 52 (2022), pp. 2391-2403
[Makino et al., 2021]
T. Makino, H. Umegaki, M. Ando, X.W. Cheng, K. Ishida, H. Akima, M. …Kuzuya.
Effects of aerobic, resistance, or combined exercise training among older adults with subjective memory complaints: A randomized controlled trial.
Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 82 (2021), pp. 701-717
[Martínez-Velilla et al., 2021]
N. Martínez-Velilla, P.L. Valenzuela, M.L. Sáez de Asteasu, F. Zambom-Ferraresi, R. Ramírez-Vélez, A. García-Hermoso, A. …Lucia.
Effects of a tailored exercise intervention in acutely hospitalized oldest old diabetic adults: an ancillary analysis.
The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 106 (2021), pp. e899-e906
[Marzolini et al., 2013]
S. Marzolini, P. Oh, W. McIlroy, D. Brooks.
The effects of an aerobic and resistance exercise training program on cognition following stroke.
Neurorehabilitation and neural repair, 27 (2013), pp. 392-402
[Mavros et al., 2017]
Y. Mavros, N. Gates, G.C. Wilson, N. Jain, J. Meiklejohn, H. Brodaty, M.A. Fiatarone Singh.
Mediation of cognitive function improvements by strength gains after resistance training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: outcomes of the study of mental and resistance training.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 65 (2017), pp. 550-559
[McArdle et al., 2010]
W.D. McArdle, F.I. Katch, V.L. Katch.
Exercise physiology: nutrition, energy, and human performance.
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, (2010),
[McAuley and Rudolph, 1995]
E. McAuley, D. Rudolph.
Physical activity, aging, and psychological well-being.
Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 3 (1995), pp. 67-96
[McCartney et al., 2021]
D. McCartney, B. Desbrow, C. Irwin.
Cognitive effects of acute aerobic exercise: exploring the influence of exercise duration, exhaustion, task complexity and expectancies in endurance-trained individuals.
Journal of sports sciences, 39 (2021), pp. 183-191
[McCrae and Sutin, 2018]
R.R. McCrae, A.R. Sutin.
A five–factor theory perspective on causal analysis.
European journal of personality, 32 (2018), pp. 151-166
[McDonnell et al., 2011]
M.N. McDonnell, A.E. Smith, S.F. Mackintosh.
Aerobic exercise to improve cognitive function in adults with neurological disorders: a systematic review.
Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation, 92 (2011), pp. 1044-1052
[McGuinness and Higgins, 2020]
L.A. McGuinness, J.P.T. Higgins.
Risk-of-bias VISualization (robvis): an R package and shiny web app for visualizing risk-of-bias assessments.
Research Synthesis Methods, n/a (2020),
[McMorris, 2020]
T. McMorris.
Cognitive fatigue effects on physical performance: the role of interoception.
Sports Medicine, 50 (2020), pp. 1703-1708
[McMorris et al., 2011]
T. McMorris, J. Sproule, A. Turner, B.J. Hale.
Acute, intermediate intensity exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: a meta-analytical comparison of effects.
Physiology & behavior, 102 (2011), pp. 421-428
[McNerney and Radvansky, 2015]
M.W. McNerney, G.A. Radvansky.
Mind racing: the influence of exercise on long-term memory consolidation.
Memory (Hove, England), 23 (2015), pp. 1140-1151
[Meijer et al., 2020]
A. Meijer, M. Königs, I.M. van der Fels, C. Visscher, R.J. Bosker, E. Hartman, J. Oosterlaan.
The effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise interventions on executive functioning in school-aged children: A cluster-randomized controlled trial.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 43 (2020), pp. 1-13
[Munguía-Izquierdo and Legaz-Arrese, 2008]
D. Munguía-Izquierdo, A. Legaz-Arrese.
Assessment of the effects of aquatic therapy on global symptomatology in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome: a randomized controlled trial.
Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation, 89 (2008), pp. 2250-2257
[Nagamatsu et al., 2012]
L.S. Nagamatsu, T.C. Handy, C.L. Hsu, M. Voss, T. Liu-Ambrose.
Resistance training promotes cognitive and functional brain plasticity in seniors with probable mild cognitive impairment.
Archives of internal medicine, 172 (2012), pp. 666-668
[Napoli et al., 2014]
N. Napoli, K. Shah, D.L. Waters, D.R. Sinacore, C. Qualls, D.T. Villareal.
Effect of weight loss, exercise, or both on cognition and quality of life in obese older adults.
The American journal of clinical nutrition, 100 (2014), pp. 189-198
[Nasreddine et al., 2005]
Z.S. Nasreddine, N.A. Phillips, V. Bédirian, S. Charbonneau, V. Whitehead, I. Collin, H. …Chertkow.
The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53 (2005), pp. 695-699
[Netz, 2019]
Y. Netz.
Is there a preferred mode of exercise for cognition enhancement in older age?—A narrative review.
Frontiers in Medicine, 6 (2019), pp. 57
[Netz et al., 2007]
Y. Netz, R. Tomer, S. Axelrad, E. Argov, O. Inbar.
The effect of a single aerobic training session on cognitive flexibility in late middle-aged adults.
International Journal of Sports Medicine, 28 (2007), pp. 82-87
[Nishiguchi et al., 2015]
S. Nishiguchi, M. Yamada, T. Tanigawa, K. Sekiyama, T. Kawagoe, M. Suzuki, T. …Tsuboyama.
A 12-week physical and cognitive exercise program can improve cognitive function and neural efficiency in community-dwelling older adults: A randomized controlled trial.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 63 (2015), pp. 1355-1363
[Northey et al., 2018]
J.M. Northey, N. Cherbuin, K.L. Pumpa, D.J. Smee, B. Rattray.
Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis.
[Norton et al., 2010]
K. Norton, L. Norton, D. Sadgrove.
Position statement on physical activity and exercise intensity terminology.
Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 13 (2010), pp. 496-502
[Nouchi and Kawashima, 2014]
R. Nouchi, R. Kawashima.
Improving cognitive function from children to old age: a systematic review of recent smart ageing intervention studies.
Advances in Neuroscience, 2014 (2014),
[Nuechterlein et al., 2018]
K. Nuechterlein, S. McEwen, J. Ventura, K. Subotnik, L. Turner, M. Boucher, J. …Hayata.
Aerobic exercise enhances cognitive training effects in first episode schizophrenia: cognitive and functional gains and promising biological mechanisms of action.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44 (2018), pp. S18
[Nuechterlein et al., 2023]
K.H. Nuechterlein, S.C. McEwen, J. Ventura, K.L. Subotnik, L.R. Turner, M. Boucher, J.N. Hayata.
Aerobic exercise enhances cognitive training effects in first-episode schizophrenia: randomized clinical trial demonstrates cognitive and functional gains.
Psychological medicine, 53 (2023), pp. 4751-4761
[Okumiya et al., 1996]
K. Okumiya, K. Matsubayashi, T. Wada, S. Kimura, Y. Doi, T. Ozawa.
Effects of exercise on neurobehavioral function in community-dwelling older people more than 75 years of age.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 44 (1996), pp. 569-572
[Opdebeeck et al., 2016]
C. Opdebeeck, A. Martyr, L. Clare.
Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: a meta-analysis.
Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 23 (2016), pp. 40-60
[Organization, 2015]
Organization, W.H. (2015). World Report on ageing and Health.
[Organization, 2021]
Organization, W.H. (2021). Global status report on the public health response to dementia.
[Paillard, 2015]
T. Paillard.
Preventive effects of regular physical exercise against cognitive decline and the risk of dementia with age advancement.
Sports Medicine-Open, 1 (2015), pp. 1-6
[Pajonk et al., 2010]
F.-G. Pajonk, T. Wobrock, O. Gruber, H. Scherk, D. Berner, I. Kaizl, T. …Meyer.
Hippocampal plasticity in response to exercise in schizophrenia.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 67 (2010), pp. 133-143
[Papatsimpas et al., 2023]
V. Papatsimpas, S. Vrouva, M. Papadopoulou, G. Papathanasiou, D. Bakalidou.
The effects of aerobic and resistance exercises on the cognitive and physical function of persons with mild dementia: A randomized controlled trial protocol.
Healthcare, 11 (2023), pp. 677
[Paterson and Warburton, 2010]
D.H. Paterson, D.E. Warburton.
Physical activity and functional limitations in older adults: a systematic review related to Canada's Physical Activity Guidelines.
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 7 (2010), pp. 1-22
[Penedo and Dahn, 2005]
F.J. Penedo, J.R. Dahn.
Exercise and well-being: a review of mental and physical health benefits associated with physical activity.
Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 18 (2005), pp. 189-193
[Peng et al., 2017]
J. Peng, X. Deng, W. Huang, J.-h. Yu, J.-x. Wang, J.-p. Wang, Y. …Zhang.
Irisin protects against neuronal injury induced by oxygen-glucose deprivation in part depends on the inhibition of ROS-NLRP3 inflammatory signaling pathway.
Molecular Immunology, 91 (2017), pp. 185-194
[Pereira et al., 2007]
A.C. Pereira, D.E. Huddleston, A.M. Brickman, A.A. Sosunov, R. Hen, G.M. McKhann, S.A. Small.
An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104 (2007), pp. 5638-5643
[Piercy et al., 2018]
K.L. Piercy, R.P. Troiano, R.M. Ballard, S.A. Carlson, J.E. Fulton, D.A. Galuska, R.D. …Olson.
The physical activity guidelines for Americans.
JAMA, 320 (2018), pp. 2020-2028
[Pontifex et al., 2009]
M.B. Pontifex, C.H. Hillman, B. Fernhall, K.M. Thompson, T.A. Valentini.
The effect of acute aerobic and resistance exercise on working memory.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 41 (2009), pp. 927-934
[Ratamess, 2021]
N. Ratamess.
ACSM's foundations of strength training and conditioning.
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, (2021),
[Ren et al., 2024]
F.-F. Ren, C.H. Hillman, W.-G. Wang, R.-H. Li, W.-S. Zhou, W.-M. Liang, Y.-K. …Chang.
Effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in adults with major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 24 (2024),
[Ren et al., 2023]
F.F. Ren, B.L. Alderman, W.G. Wang, F.T. Chen, W.S. Zhou, W.J. Zong, Y.K. Chang.
Effects of exercise training on executive functioning in adults with depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials.
Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.), 53 (2023), pp. 1765-1788
[Robinson et al., 2023]
K. Robinson, N. Riley, K. Owen, R. Drew, M.F. Mavilidi, C.H. Hillman, D.R. Lubans.
Effects of resistance training on academic outcomes in school-aged youth: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Sports Medicine, (2023), pp. 1-15
[Rohde and Thompson, 2007]
T.E. Rohde, L.A. Thompson.
Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability.
Intelligence, 35 (2007), pp. 83-92
[Romberg et al., 2005]
A. Romberg, A. Virtanen, J. Ruutiainen.
Long–term exercise improves functional impairment but not quality of life in multiple sclerosis.
Journal of Neurology, 252 (2005), pp. 839-845
[Rowe and Kahn, 1997]
J.W. Rowe, R.L. Kahn.
Successful aging.
The Gerontologist, 37 (1997), pp. 433-440
[Ruiz et al., 2015]
J.R. Ruiz, F. Gil-Bea, N. Bustamante-Ara, G. Rodríguez-Romo, C. Fiuza-Luces, J.A. Serra-Rexach, A. …Lucia.
Resistance training does not have an effect on cognition or related serum biomarkers in nonagenarians: A randomized controlled trial.
International Journal of Sports Medicine, 36 (2015), pp. 54-60
[Ruiz et al., 2015]
J.R. Ruiz, F. Gil-Bea, N. Bustamante-Ara, G. Rodríguez-Romo, C. Fiuza-Luces, J.A. Serra-Rexach, A. …Lucia.
Resistance training does not have an effect on cognition or related serum biomarkers in nonagenarians: a randomized controlled trial.
International Journal of Sports Medicine, 36 (2015), pp. 54-60
[Sandroff et al., 2015]
B.M. Sandroff, C.H. Hillman, R.H. Benedict, R.W. Motl.
Acute effects of walking, cycling, and yoga exercise on cognition in persons with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis without impaired cognitive processing speed.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 37 (2015), pp. 209-219
[Schuch et al., 2016]
F.B. Schuch, D. Vancampfort, J. Richards, S. Rosenbaum, P.B. Ward, B. Stubbs.
Exercise as a treatment for depression: a meta-analysis adjusting for publication bias.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 77 (2016), pp. 42-51
[Schuch et al., 2016]
F.B. Schuch, D. Vancampfort, X. Sui, S. Rosenbaum, J. Firth, J. Richards, B. …Stubbs.
Are lower levels of cardiorespiratory fitness associated with incident depression? A systematic review of prospective cohort studies.
Preventive medicine, 93 (2016), pp. 159-165
[Segabinazi et al., 2020]
E. Segabinazi, N.F. Gasperini, A.M. Faustino, R. Centeno, A. Dos Santos, W.d. Almeida, L. Pereira.
Comparative overview of the effects of aerobic and resistance exercise on anxiety-like behavior, cognitive flexibility, and hippocampal synaptic plasticity parameters in healthy rats.
Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 53 (2020), pp. e9816
[Shimada et al., 2018]
H. Shimada, H. Makizako, T. Doi, H. Park, K. Tsutsumimoto, J. Verghese, T. Suzuki.
Effects of combined physical and cognitive exercises on cognition and mobility in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized clinical trial.
Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 19 (2018), pp. 584-591
[Silva et al., 2024]
N.C.B.S. Silva, C.K. Barha, K.I. Erickson, A.F. Kramer, T. Liu-Ambrose.
Physical exercise, cognition, and brain health in aging.
Trends in Neurosciences, (2024),
[Sink et al., 2015]
K.M. Sink, M.A. Espeland, C.M. Castro, T. Church, R. Cohen, J.A. Dodson, J. …Katula.
Effect of a 24-month physical activity intervention vs health education on cognitive outcomes in sedentary older adults: the LIFE randomized trial.
JAMA, 314 (2015), pp. 781-790
[Smiley-Oyen et al., 2008]
A.L. Smiley-Oyen, K.A. Lowry, S.J. Francois, M.L. Kohut, P. Ekkekakis.
Exercise, fitness, and neurocognitive function in older adults: the “selective improvement” and “cardiovascular fitness” hypotheses.
[Song et al., 2018]
D. Song, S. Doris, P.W. Li, Y. Lei.
The effectiveness of physical exercise on cognitive and psychological outcomes in individuals with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
International Journal of Nursing Studies, 79 (2018), pp. 155-164
[Stern, 2002]
Y. Stern.
What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8 (2002), pp. 448-460
[Stern et al., 2019]
Y. Stern, A. MacKay-Brandt, S. Lee, P. McKinley, K. McIntyre, Q. Razlighi, R.P. …Sloan.
Effect of aerobic exercise on cognition in younger adults: A randomized clinical trial.
[Stillman et al., 2020]
C.M. Stillman, I. Esteban-Cornejo, B. Brown, C.M. Bender, K.I. Erickson.
Effects of exercise on brain and cognition across age groups and health states.
[Stringhini et al., 2017]
S. Stringhini, C. Carmeli, M. Jokela, M. Avendaño, P. Muennig, F. Guida, M. …Bochud.
Socioeconomic status and the 25 × 25 risk factors as determinants of premature mortality: a multicohort study and meta-analysis of 1· 7 million men and women.
The Lancet, 389 (2017), pp. 1229-1237
[Stroth et al., 2009]
S. Stroth, K. Hille, M. Spitzer, R. Reinhardt.
Aerobic endurance exercise benefits memory and affect in young adults.
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 19 (2009), pp. 223-243
[Sujkowski et al., 2022]
A. Sujkowski, L. Hong, R. Wessells, S.V. Todi.
The protective role of exercise against age-related neurodegeneration.
Ageing Research Reviews, 74 (2022),
[Sun et al., 2020]
T. Sun, Y. Wang, D. Hui, X. Jing, W. Feng.
Vertical distributions of soil microbial biomass carbon: a global dataset.
Data in brief, 32 (2020),
[Suzuki et al., 2013]
T. Suzuki, H. Shimada, H. Makizako, T. Doi, D. Yoshida, K. Ito, T. …Kato.
A randomized controlled trial of multicomponent exercise in older adults with mild cognitive impairment.
[Tarazona-Santabalbina et al., 2016]
F.J. Tarazona-Santabalbina, M.C. Gómez-Cabrera, P. Pérez-Ros, F.M. Martínez-Arnau, H. Cabo, K. Tsaparas, J. …Viña.
A multicomponent exercise intervention that reverses frailty and improves cognition, emotion, and social networking in the community-dwelling frail elderly: a randomized clinical trial.
Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 17 (2016), pp. 426-433
[Tari et al., 2019]
A.R. Tari, C.S. Norevik, N.R. Scrimgeour, A. Kobro-Flatmoen, J. Storm-Mathisen, L.H. Bergersen, U. …Wisløff.
Are the neuroprotective effects of exercise training systemically mediated?.
Progress in Cardiovascular Diseases, 62 (2019), pp. 94-101
[Teixeira et al., 2012]
P.J. Teixeira, E.V. Carraça, D. Markland, M.N. Silva, R.M. Ryan.
Exercise, physical activity, and self-determination theory: a systematic review.
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 9 (2012), pp. 1-30
[Thaiyanto et al., 2021]
J. Thaiyanto, C. Sittichoke, K. Phirom, S. Sungkarat.
Effects of multicomponent exercise on cognitive performance and fall risk in older women with mild cognitive impairment.
The Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging, 25 (2021), pp. 160-164
[Timmons et al., 2018]
J.F. Timmons, D. Minnock, M. Hone, K.E. Cogan, J.C. Murphy, B. Egan.
Comparison of time-matched aerobic, resistance, or concurrent exercise training in older adults.
Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 28 (2018), pp. 2272-2283
[Tomporowski and Pesce, 2019]
P.D. Tomporowski, C. Pesce.
Exercise, sports, and performance arts benefit cognition via a common process.
Psychological Bulletin, 145 (2019), pp. 929
[Tsai et al., 2014]
C.-L. Tsai, F.-C. Chen, C.-Y. Pan, C.-H. Wang, T.-H. Huang, T.-C. Chen.
Impact of acute aerobic exercise and cardiorespiratory fitness on visuospatial attention performance and serum BDNF levels.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 41 (2014), pp. 121-131
[Tsai et al., 2019]
C.-L. Tsai, M.-C. Pai, J. Ukropec, B. Ukropcová.
Distinctive effects of aerobic and resistance exercise modes on neurocognitive and biochemical changes in individuals with mild cognitive impairment.
Current Alzheimer Research, 16 (2019), pp. 316-332
[Turrini et al., 2023]
S. Turrini, B. Wong, M. Eldaief, D. Press, D.A. Sinclair, G. Koch, E. …Santarnecchi.
The multifactorial nature of healthy brain ageing: brain changes, functional decline and protective factors.
Ageing Research Reviews, (2023),
[Viechtbauer, 2005]
W. Viechtbauer.
Bias and efficiency of meta-analytic variance estimators in the random-effects model.
Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 30 (2005), pp. 261-293
[Viechtbauer, 2010]
W. Viechtbauer.
Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package.
Journal of Statistical Software, 36 (2010),
[Voelcker-Rehage and Niemann, 2013]
C. Voelcker-Rehage, C. Niemann.
Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37 (2013), pp. 2268-2295
[Voss et al., 2013]
M.W. Voss, C. Vivar, A.F. Kramer, H. van Praag.
Bridging animal and human models of exercise-induced brain plasticity.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17 (2013), pp. 525-544
[Vreugdenhil et al., 2012]
A. Vreugdenhil, J. Cannell, A. Davies, G. Razay.
A community-based exercise programme to improve functional ability in people with Alzheimer's disease: A randomized controlled trial.
Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 26 (2012), pp. 12-19
[Wadsworth et al., 2022]
D.D. Wadsworth, M. Rodriguez-Hernandez, L.S. Huffman, J.R. McDonald, K.E. Spring, D.D. Pascoe.
Adaptations to a concurrent exercise training program in inactive aging women.
The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research, 36 (2022), pp. 3217-3223
[Welsh et al., 2010]
J.A. Welsh, R.L. Nix, C. Blair, K.L. Bierman, K.E. Nelson.
The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 102 (2010), pp. 43
[Whitfield and Hofmann, 2023]
S. Whitfield, M.A. Hofmann.
Elicit: AI literature review research assistant.
Public Services Quarterly, 19 (2023), pp. 201-207
[Wilke et al., 2020]
J. Wilke, V. Stricker, S. Usedly.
Free-weight resistance exercise is more effective in enhancing inhibitory control than machine-based training: A randomized, controlled trial.
Brain Sciences, 10 (2020), pp. 702
[Williamson et al., 2009]
J.D. Williamson, M. Espeland, S.B. Kritchevsky, A.B. Newman, A.C. King, M. Pahor, L.S. Investigators.
Changes in cognitive function in a randomized trial of physical activity: results of the lifestyle interventions and independence for elders pilot study.
Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences, 64 (2009), pp. 688-694
[Xu et al., 2021]
M. Xu, J. Zhu, X.-D. Liu, M.-Y. Luo, N.-J. Xu.
Roles of physical exercise in neurodegeneration: reversal of epigenetic clock.
Translational Neurodegeneration, 10 (2021), pp. 30
[Young et al., 2015]
J. Young, M. Angevaren, J. Rusted, N. Tabet.
Aerobic exercise to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment.
The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2015 (2015),
[Zhang et al., 2023]
M. Zhang, J. Jia, Y. Yang, L. Zhang, X. Wang.
Effects of exercise interventions on cognitive functions in healthy populations: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Ageing Research Reviews, 92 (2023),
[Zhang et al., 2022]
S. Zhang, K. Zhen, Q. Su, Y. Chen, Y. Lv, L. Yu.
The effect of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in people with Alzheimer's disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19 (2022), pp. 15700
[Zhidong et al., 2021]
C. Zhidong, X. Wang, J. Yin, D. Song, Z. Chen.
Effects of physical exercise on working memory in older adults: a systematic and meta-analytic review.
European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 18 (2021), pp. 1-15
[Zimmerman et al., 2014]
B. Zimmerman, B.P. Sutton, K.A. Low, M.A. Fletcher, C.H. Tan, N. Schneider-Garces, G. …Gratton.
Cardiorespiratory fitness mediates the effects of aging on cerebral blood flow.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6 (2014), pp. 59
[Zou et al., 2024]
L. Zou, F. Herold, B. Cheval, M.J. Wheeler, D.M. Pindus, K.I. Erickson, N. …Owen.
Sedentary behavior and lifespan brain health.
Trends in cognitive sciences, 28 (2024), pp. 369-382

Mingyang Zhang will handle correspondence at all stages of submission, including revision and publication.

Descargar PDF
Opciones de artículo